1999
DOI: 10.1080/036012799267585
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Child and Adolescent Knowledge and Attitudes About Older Adults Across Time and States

Abstract: The original P almore' s F acts on A ging Quiz was modi ed and presented to 954 elementary, middle, and high school students (grades 3, 6, 9, 12) in West V irginia and Texas. Three separate experiments were summariz ed involving the Child-A dolescent F acts on A ging Quiz (CA F A Q). Similar student responses on number of correct items and bias were noted in 1983 and 1998. A dolescents showed positive bias, but elementary school children showed negative bias toward older adults. F emale adolescents tended to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Even Kwong See and Nicoladis' (2010) study on 2-to 3-year-old children showed that those with more frequent contacts with their grandparents judged the elderly more favorably in their capacity to teach the meaning of new words. However, many other studies did not find significant correlations between the frequency of youth-elderly contacts and the youth's views on the seniors (Boswell, 2012;Haught et al, 1999;Okoye, 2005).…”
Section: Contacts With the Elderlymentioning
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Even Kwong See and Nicoladis' (2010) study on 2-to 3-year-old children showed that those with more frequent contacts with their grandparents judged the elderly more favorably in their capacity to teach the meaning of new words. However, many other studies did not find significant correlations between the frequency of youth-elderly contacts and the youth's views on the seniors (Boswell, 2012;Haught et al, 1999;Okoye, 2005).…”
Section: Contacts With the Elderlymentioning
confidence: 83%
“…All studies are congruent about the influence of gender on children's ageist views: Boys of all ages hold more negative visions of aging and the elderly than girls (Allan & Johnson, 2009), expressing aversion to getting old by using words such as "it is terrible" (Zandi, Mirle, & Jarvis, 1990). With regard to age, although results between studies have been divergent, they generally demonstrate a worsening of the ageist stereotypes with age in early childhood, between 4 and 7 years (Blunk & Williams, 1997;Burke, 1982), and a regression in late adolescence (Haught, Walls, Laney, Leavell, & Stuzen, 1999). In a sample of 6-to 14-year-old boys and girls, the lowest level of ageist stereotypes was found around 9-to 10-years (Lineweaver, Roy, & Horth, 2017).…”
Section: Impact Of Gender and Age On Ageismmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Respondents indicated aging knowledge with a 16-item Facts on Aging Quiz (Haught, Walls, Laney, Leavell, & Stuzen, 1999). Respondents indicated whether an item was true or false, and the number of correctly answered items was summed (a ¼ .36).…”
Section: Methods Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We suspect that the design of the scale as a multi-dimensional measure of aging knowledge results in underestimation of its reliability (Schmitt, 1996). Despite its low internal consistency, we included the scale in our analyses because it demonstrates predictive utility with a young adult population (Haught et al, 1999) and because knowledge about aging has been associated with one's attitudes towards aging (e.g., Funderburk, Damron-Rodriguez, Storms, & Solomon, 2006). We would use an alternate measure with greater reliability in a replication study before abandoning the idea that greater knowledge about an outgroup contributes to less aging selfambivalence.…”
Section: Study Limitations and Next Stepsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, attitudes about aging begin to emerge in early childhood, when older adults are perceived as a distant out-group, but as individuals proceed through the life span, their views and perceptions about aging become increasingly self-relevant and self-fulfilling (Haught, Walls, Laney, Leavell, & Stuzen, 1999;Hawkins, 1996;Scott, Minichiello, & Browning, 1998). Specifically, attitudes about aging begin to emerge in early childhood, when older adults are perceived as a distant out-group, but as individuals proceed through the life span, their views and perceptions about aging become increasingly self-relevant and self-fulfilling (Haught, Walls, Laney, Leavell, & Stuzen, 1999;Hawkins, 1996;Scott, Minichiello, & Browning, 1998).…”
Section: Attitudes Toward Agingmentioning
confidence: 99%