2000
DOI: 10.1016/s0167-8760(99)00105-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERP) as a key to the psychology of odors

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

8
88
2

Year Published

2001
2001
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 134 publications
(99 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
8
88
2
Order By: Relevance
“…MMN is resistant to manipulations of attention and states of wakefulness (Sculthorpe et al, 2009) even though these parameters can modulate its amplitude. An analog of MMN was described in visual (Tales et al, 1999;Pazo-Alvarez et al, 2003), olfactive (Krauel et al, 1999;Pause and Krauel, 2000), and somatosensory (Kekoni et al, 1997;Shinozaki et al, 1998) modalities, supporting a broad computational significance of MMN as a shared and automatic brain mechanism responsive to stimulus novelty.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…MMN is resistant to manipulations of attention and states of wakefulness (Sculthorpe et al, 2009) even though these parameters can modulate its amplitude. An analog of MMN was described in visual (Tales et al, 1999;Pazo-Alvarez et al, 2003), olfactive (Krauel et al, 1999;Pause and Krauel, 2000), and somatosensory (Kekoni et al, 1997;Shinozaki et al, 1998) modalities, supporting a broad computational significance of MMN as a shared and automatic brain mechanism responsive to stimulus novelty.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…As noted by Friston et al (2005), the closely similar neuronal architecture of cortical layers throughout the cerebral cortex supports the view that a similar computational principle of predictive coding may apply to the multiple hierarchical levels of the cortical areas of the brain. Thus, our model may be used to account for higher-order instances of mismatch responses, such as the distinct MMNs evoked by a change in phoneme versus speaker (Giard et al, 1995;Dehaene-Lambertz, 1997), or the mismatch responses observed outside the auditory modality, either in visual (Tales et al, 1999;Pazo-Alvarez et al, 2003), olfactive (Krauel et al, 1999;Pause and Krauel, 2000), and somatosensory (Kekoni et al, 1997;Shinozaki et al, 1998) modalities or even in a crossmodal context (Arnal et al, 2011).…”
Section: Extensions and Limits Of The Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Pleasantness is the primary perceptual aspect humans use to discriminate odorants (Schiffman, 1974;Godinot and Sicard, 1995) or combine them into groups (Berglund et al, 1973;Schiffman et al, 1977). Pleasant and unpleasant odorants are evaluated at different speeds (Bensafi et al, 2002) and by dissociable neural substrates, as evidenced in both electrophysiological recordings (Kobal et al, 1992;Pause and Krauel, 2000;Masago et al, 2001) and functional neuroimaging studies (Zald and Pardo, 1997;Royet et al, 2000;Gottfried et al, 2002;Anderson et al, 2003;Rolls et al, 2003). Finally, studies with newborns suggest that at least some aspects of olfactory pleasantness may be innate (Steiner, 1979;Soussignan et al, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The early, exogenous components, of the OERP reflect the external stimulus and various studies have demonstrated that the N1P2 amplitude is dependent upon the stimulus concentration (Kobal and Hummel, 1991;Tateyama et al, 1998;Wang et al, 2002). The later positive components (1000-1500 ms poststimulus) are thought to reflect cognitive operations (Rösler et al, 1986; Vergeler, Learning to smell steroids N Boulkroune et al 1988; Pause and Krauel, 2000). We show that there are increases in both the early (P1N1) and late (N1P2) components of the OERP (Figure 1e and d), with a pronounced increase in the first positive peak (P1).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%