2009
DOI: 10.1080/08824090903074373
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Changes in Teasing Patterns From Early Adolescence to Adolescence

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Along the same lines, we observed that the 16-year-olds used more inclusive irony than the 12-year-olds. Th ese exploratory observations about the use of inclusive or exclusive irony during adolescence are consistent with the work of Sherer and Clark (2009), who showed that as teenagers grow older, teasing is most oft en initiated to have fun, and with studies demonstrating that bullying behaviors decrease with age in adolescence, aft er a peak when pupils move up from primary to secondary school (Griffi n & Gross, 2004). Th ey are also consistent with the work of Pexman, Glenwright, Krol, and James (2005), who found that the humorous and teasing functions of irony are not well understood in late childhood (i.e., 7-to 10-year-olds).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Along the same lines, we observed that the 16-year-olds used more inclusive irony than the 12-year-olds. Th ese exploratory observations about the use of inclusive or exclusive irony during adolescence are consistent with the work of Sherer and Clark (2009), who showed that as teenagers grow older, teasing is most oft en initiated to have fun, and with studies demonstrating that bullying behaviors decrease with age in adolescence, aft er a peak when pupils move up from primary to secondary school (Griffi n & Gross, 2004). Th ey are also consistent with the work of Pexman, Glenwright, Krol, and James (2005), who found that the humorous and teasing functions of irony are not well understood in late childhood (i.e., 7-to 10-year-olds).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Research by Jones, Newman, and Bautista (2005) suggests that children consider the relationship they have with the teaser when interpreting the meaning of tease, a tendency that they labeled the ''positivity bias'' (p. 433). However, we do not yet know whether that bias holds true from early childhood through adolescence, particularly given in Sherer and Clark's (2009) finding that from sixth grade through high school, the rate of teasing friends increased dramatically, from 10% to 60%, yet the increase was not accompanied by a decrease in negative teasing of disliked peers with advancing age. As children age, increasingly complex ways of communicating are necessary to comment on social life.…”
Section: Relationship Developmentmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Academic interest in teasing in children’s social relationships has been driven by social crises such as school shootings (Kowalski, 2003; Leary, Kowalski, Smith, & Phillips, 2003), and self-directed harm such as suicidal thoughts (Eisenberg, Neumark-Sztainer, & Story, 2003). In contrast to the dark side of teasing, other scholars provide evidence that teasing can be a positive force in children and adolescents’ social relationships (Campos, Keltner, Beck, Gonzaga, & John, 2007; Sherer & Clark, 2009). Research has shown that teasing increases positive interactions in families (Eisenberg, 1986), prepares children for later life challenges (Miller, 1986), helps build friendships (Shapiro, Baumeister, & Kessler, 1991), and is used to manage conflict (Eder, Evans, & Parker, 1995).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%