1954
DOI: 10.1007/bf02289188
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Changes in factor structure of a complex psychomotor test as a function of practice

Abstract: Scores obtained at eight different stages of practice on the Complex Coordination Test together with scores on 18 reference tests were subjected to a Thurstone Centroid Factor Analysis. Nine meaningful factors were identified in the experimental battery. The results indlea~ed considerable, but systematic, changes in the factor structure of the Complex Coordination Test as practice on the task was continued. The test became less complex (faetorially) as practice was continued. Moreover, there was a change in th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
22
0
1

Year Published

1964
1964
1995
1995

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 217 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
1
22
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We think the capacity demands of the interpolated task Crowder (1967a) used were quite different from those used both in the present experiments and in others investigating decay in short-term memory (especially the detection tasks of Reitman and Shiffrin). It may well be that with Crowder's more complex interpolated task it cannot be assumed that the task is strictly nonverbal, especially at low levels of practice (Fleishman & Hemple, 1954). Under conditions of low stimulus-response compatibility and practice, it is quite likely that subjects used verbal rules to mediate performance ("third light from the left goes with second key from the right," and so forth).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We think the capacity demands of the interpolated task Crowder (1967a) used were quite different from those used both in the present experiments and in others investigating decay in short-term memory (especially the detection tasks of Reitman and Shiffrin). It may well be that with Crowder's more complex interpolated task it cannot be assumed that the task is strictly nonverbal, especially at low levels of practice (Fleishman & Hemple, 1954). Under conditions of low stimulus-response compatibility and practice, it is quite likely that subjects used verbal rules to mediate performance ("third light from the left goes with second key from the right," and so forth).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The labels for psychomotor constructs in Table I are derived from the classic work of Fleishman and his colleagues (e.g., Fleishman, 1954;Fleishman & Ellison, 1962;Fleishman & Hempel, 1954a, 1954b, 1956), Imhoff andLevine (1981), andMcHenry (1987). The more recent works have focused on hierarchical models of psychomotor abilities--models that are compatible with Fleishman's taxonomy.…”
Section: Psychomotormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…During World War II, the Army Air Force (AAF) studies made great strides toward defining and measuring psychomotor abilities (Guilford & Lacey, 1947). Fleishman and his colleagues continued psychomotor abilities research in the 1950s and early 1960s (e.g., Fleishman, 1967Fleishman, , 1972Fleishman & Hempel, 1954a, 1954b, 1956). Fleishman performed a series of factor analytic studies with military airmen and airmen trainees to identify the basic structure of the psychomotor domain (Fleishman, 1954;Fleishman & Ellison, 1962;Fleishman & Hempel, 1954a, 1956).…”
Section: Psychomotor Attributes Psychomotor Attribute Definitionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Seashore (1951) has indicated that the initial rate of progress in motor skills is significantly and positively correlated with the ultimate performance level. The justification for the assessment of skill acquisition rate is further substantiated by the change in factor structure of complex psychomotor tasks as function of practice (Fleishman & Hempel, 1953) and evidence that measures of the initial performance on complex psychomotor tasks are not valid predictors of final performance or performance on a criterion task (Adams, 1953;Smith & Gold, 1956;Parker & Fleishman, 1960). Fleishman and Rich (1963) stated that individuals who have superior sensitivity to kinesthetic cues should be superior to others at the advanced stages of leasrning of a complex motor task, but they may not excel during the initial stages.…”
Section: Critical Behavioral Functionsmentioning
confidence: 99%