2020
DOI: 10.1101/2020.11.29.403139
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Change point detection with multiple alternatives reveals parallel evaluation of the same stream of evidence along distinct timescales

Abstract: In order to behave appropriately in a rapidly changing world, individuals must be able to detect when changes occur in that environment. However, at any given moment, there are a multitude of potential changes of behavioral significance that could occur. Here we investigate how knowledge about the space of possible changes affects human change point detection. We used a stochastic auditory change point detection task that allowed model-free and model-based characterization of the decision process people employ… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
2
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
1
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In particular, the well-known P300 component has long been argued to be associated with detection of statistical surprise (Donchin, 1981;Duncan-Johnson and Donchin, 1977;Mars et al, 2008;Squires et al, 1976) or, more recently, a correlate of a continuous time-evolving decision variable (Twomey et al, 2015) that is equivalent to the centroparietal positivity (Kelly and O'Connell, 2013;O'Connell et al, 2012). Our centroparietal responses to |Devidence| (fig 4b) are consistent with a changepoint detection account of continuous decision making (Booras et al, 2021), in which decision-relevant input (fig 6) is evaluated for a change in latent state from a baseline period to a response period (Nassar et al, 2019). This account would also explain why these signals are enhanced when response periods are rarer, as a large |Devidence| is more statistically surprising when response periods are rare than when they are common.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…In particular, the well-known P300 component has long been argued to be associated with detection of statistical surprise (Donchin, 1981;Duncan-Johnson and Donchin, 1977;Mars et al, 2008;Squires et al, 1976) or, more recently, a correlate of a continuous time-evolving decision variable (Twomey et al, 2015) that is equivalent to the centroparietal positivity (Kelly and O'Connell, 2013;O'Connell et al, 2012). Our centroparietal responses to |Devidence| (fig 4b) are consistent with a changepoint detection account of continuous decision making (Booras et al, 2021), in which decision-relevant input (fig 6) is evaluated for a change in latent state from a baseline period to a response period (Nassar et al, 2019). This account would also explain why these signals are enhanced when response periods are rarer, as a large |Devidence| is more statistically surprising when response periods are rare than when they are common.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…3D-F been demonstrated that different evidence integration strategies can be explained by specific differences in behavioural paradigms. Among important features that influence weighting profiles are the division of evidence during a trial (Bronfman et al, 2016;Levi et al, 2018;Raposo et al, 2012) and choice expectation (Booras et al, 2021;Talluri et al, 2021). When stimulus information is equally informative throughout the trial (Kiani et al, 2008;Levi et al, 2018) and the observer is able to report the decision whenever they have solved the task, like in the paradigm used here, there is no need to integrate late information after the decision has been made.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Different strategies have been revealed for studying sensory evidence accumulation. A number of studies using fluctuating visual information (i.e., where the visual stimulus reliability changes over time) have demonstrated that observers tend to weight early sensory information more heavily than late information (Huk & Shadlen, 2005;Kiani et al, 2008;Nienborg & Cumming, 2009;Zylberberg et al, 2012;Odoemene et al, 2018;Booras et al, 2021). However, late sensory information integration strategies (Bronfman et al, 2016;Cheadle et al, 2014;Levi et al, 2018) and flat weighting profiles (Bronfman et al, 2016;Odoemene et al, 2018) have also been observed.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%