1999
DOI: 10.1097/00001756-199909090-00026
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cerebral organization in bilinguals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
16
2

Year Published

2000
2000
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 155 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
3
16
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Recent brain-imaging studies on the cortical representation of an L2 are not univocal. Although some studies argue for different neural tissue being involved in L1 and L2 processing (43,44), others report a shared neural substrate (45). One attempt to unify these opposing results has been to propose that not only the age of acquisition but particularly the proficiency level determines the amount of cortical overlap involved in L1 and L2 processing (10).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent brain-imaging studies on the cortical representation of an L2 are not univocal. Although some studies argue for different neural tissue being involved in L1 and L2 processing (43,44), others report a shared neural substrate (45). One attempt to unify these opposing results has been to propose that not only the age of acquisition but particularly the proficiency level determines the amount of cortical overlap involved in L1 and L2 processing (10).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Originally discovered in patients with brain damage (e.g., Broca, 1861; Dax, 1863; Wernicke, 1874/1969; Geschwind, 1970), these regions have been observed in PET and fMRI since the earliest days of brain imaging research (e.g., Petersen et al, 1988; Petersen and Fiez, 1993; Binder et al, 1997). These regions are consistent (albeit variable in their exact topography; e.g., Fedorenko et al, 2010) across individuals (e.g., Frost et al, 1999; Allendorfer et al, 2012), languages (e.g., Chee et al, 1999a,b; Illes et al, 1999; Klein et al, 1999; Hernandez et al, 2001; Pu et al, 2001; Hasegawa et al, 2002; Chee et al, 2003; Mahendra et al, 2003; Briellmann et al, 2004; see e.g., van Heuven and Dijkstra, 2010 and Sebastian et al, 2011 for reviews), modality of presentation (e.g., Chee et al, 1999c; Pinel et al, 2007; Buchweitz et al, 2009; Fedorenko et al, 2010; Braze et al, 2011) and developmental experiences, including complete sensory deprivation in the auditory or visual modality (Neville et al, 1998; Newman et al, 2010; Bedny et al, 2011). Furthermore, these regions can be quickly (in ~10–15 min) and reliably identified in individual participants (Fedorenko et al, 2010), and they are stable within an individual over time (Figure 1B; Fedorenko et al, 2010; Mahowald and Fedorenko, in preparation), as well as being robust to changes in the materials, modality of presentation, and task (Figure 1C), and language for bilingual speakers (Figure 1D).…”
Section: High-level Language Processing Brain Regions and Domain-genementioning
confidence: 91%
“…Evidence for the DP (and other similar) models includes behavioral (psycholinguistic) (Bowden et al, 2010; Neubauer and Clahsen, 2009; Silva and Clahsen, 2008) and neuroimaging studies of L1 and L2 learners (e.g., Golestani and Zatorre, 2004; Xue et al, 2004). For example, neuroimaging studies suggest that lexical processing in L1 and L2 relies on similar neural substrates (Chee et al, 1999; Klein et al, 1999; Xue et al, 2004), but that the neural substrates underlying L1 and L2 grammatical processing differ either qualitatively or quantitatively, with the area and level of activation becoming more L1-like (Golestani and Zatorre, 2004; Perani et al, 1998) and more specific to inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Abutalebi, 2008) at higher levels of proficiency.…”
Section: Language Learning and Memory Systemsmentioning
confidence: 99%