1998
DOI: 10.1121/1.421353
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

“Central” auditory gap detection: A spatial case

Abstract: Normal listeners were tested for their temporal auditory gap detection thresholds using free-field presentation of white-noise stimuli delivered from the left (L) and right (R) poles of the interaural axis. The noise bursts serving as the leading and trailing markers for the silent period were presented in either the same (LL,RR) or different (LR,RL) auditory locations. The duration of the leading marker was a second independent variable. Gap thresholds for stimuli in which the markers had the same location we… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

9
39
0
11

Year Published

2005
2005
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
9
39
0
11
Order By: Relevance
“…Also interesting but requiring further exploration were the interaction effects suggesting activity associated with specific channel condition and gap duration combinations for particular temporal and spatial factor combinations. These data are consistent with the existence of a central timing mechanism that must monitor the activity in a variety of temporal and spatial regions to detect a gap between spectrally different markers (e.g., Phillips, Hall, Harrington, & Taylor, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…Also interesting but requiring further exploration were the interaction effects suggesting activity associated with specific channel condition and gap duration combinations for particular temporal and spatial factor combinations. These data are consistent with the existence of a central timing mechanism that must monitor the activity in a variety of temporal and spatial regions to detect a gap between spectrally different markers (e.g., Phillips, Hall, Harrington, & Taylor, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 68%
“…The reason for this is not clear, although it probably reflects the fact that our listeners were more practiced than his at this task. The thresholds are, however, comparable to those in previous reports from this laboratory, using different listeners (Phillips et al, 1998;Phillips et al, 1997). The important point to take from this experiment is that gap detection thresholds were most strongly affected by the similarity with which the markers activated the auditory periphery.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 66%
“…Similarly, gap thresholds were 5-10 times longer when the markers bounding the silent period originated from free-field sources opposite each ear (i.e., at Ϯ90º azimuth) than when both of the markers originated from the same source Phillips, Hall, Harrington, & Taylor, 1998). Two sounds originating from different free-field spatial locations result in different values of the binaural cues, which lead to lateralized percepts.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Effects of pre-gap durations on behavioral gap detection have been reported for durations shorter than 500 ms (Penner, 1977;Forrest and Green, 1987;Phillips et al, 1998;Schneider and Hamstra, 1999;Snell and Hu, 1999) showing that thresholds are unaffected by pre-gap durations of a few hundred ms. A study on the electrophysiological correlates of gap detection with pre-gap durations of 5, 20 and 50 ms found correlations between the detection thresholds and the Middle-Latency fields (Rupp et al, 2004). The effects on the N-Complex observed in this study may have behavioral accompaniments, more subtle than gap detection thresholds, that have not been studied yet.…”
Section: The N-complex As a Function Of Pre-gap Conditionsmentioning
confidence: 99%