2019
DOI: 10.1080/09515089.2018.1564025
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Causal deviance and the ascription of intent and blame

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Responsibility attribution is central to “everyday reasoning” and is so compelling a concept that people even invent responsibility where none exists (Iyengar, 1994, p. 9). Blame tends to be associated with events that are seen as intentional and also where the outcomes of the action are foreseeable (Alicke, 2000; Ames & Fiske, 2015; R. Rogers et al, 2019).…”
Section: Covid-19 “Origin Beliefs” and Blame Attributionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Responsibility attribution is central to “everyday reasoning” and is so compelling a concept that people even invent responsibility where none exists (Iyengar, 1994, p. 9). Blame tends to be associated with events that are seen as intentional and also where the outcomes of the action are foreseeable (Alicke, 2000; Ames & Fiske, 2015; R. Rogers et al, 2019).…”
Section: Covid-19 “Origin Beliefs” and Blame Attributionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some theorists argue that these effects result from people’s tendency to blame agents (Alicke, 1992; Alicke, Mandel, Hilton, Gerstenberg, & Lagnado, 2015; Alicke, Rose, & Bloom, 2011; Rogers et al, 2019; Shaver, 1985). On these views, people’s desire to blame a person biases their causal judgments (Alicke, 1992; Rogers et al, 2019). Someone who violates a norm is more blameworthy than someone who does not, and this desire to blame such an individual makes people more inclined to identify the actions of that individual as the cause of the outcome.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Only modal explanations, however, predict that they should. On social cognition views, people align their causal judgments with their desire to blame the source of their reactions (Alicke, 1992; Rogers et al, 2019). So, if the undesirable outcome has not yet occurred, then there is no reaction that motivates the alignment of a causal judgment with their desire to blame.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are several explanations for why people select norm-violating factors as causes. Some theorists argue that these effects result from people's tendency to blame agents (Alicke, 1992;Alicke, Rose, & Bloom, 2011;Alicke et al, 2015;Rogers et al, 2019;Shaver, 1985). On these views, people's desire to blame a person biases their causal judgments (Alicke, 1992;Rogers et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some theorists argue that these effects result from people's tendency to blame agents (Alicke, 1992;Alicke, Rose, & Bloom, 2011;Alicke et al, 2015;Rogers et al, 2019;Shaver, 1985). On these views, people's desire to blame a person biases their causal judgments (Alicke, 1992;Rogers et al, 2019). Someone who violates a norm is more blameworthy than someone who does not, and this desire to blame such an individual makes people more inclined to identify the actions of that individual as the cause of the outcome.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%