1994
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.20.3.591
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Catching balls: How to get the hand to the right place at the right time.

Abstract: Information specifying the future passing distance of an approaching object is available (in units of object size) in the ratio of optical displacement velocity and optical expansion velocity. Despite empirical support for the assumption that object size can serve as a metric in the perception of passing distance, the present series of experiments reveals that in catching a ball subjects do not rely on such "point-predictive" information. The angle at which (real and simulated) balls approached the subject sys… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

16
297
1
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 238 publications
(324 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
(98 reference statements)
16
297
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Even though this hypothesis has been much disputed in recent studies of the control of interceptive actions (e.g., Bootsma et al, 1997;Dessing, Bullock, Peper, & Beek, 2002;Lee et al, 1983;Montagne et al, 2001;Peper, Bootsma, Mestre, & Bakker, 1994;Savelsbergh et al, 1991), it must be the case that the specific characteristics of this task require a prediction, as control of the action is not possible after releasing the ball.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even though this hypothesis has been much disputed in recent studies of the control of interceptive actions (e.g., Bootsma et al, 1997;Dessing, Bullock, Peper, & Beek, 2002;Lee et al, 1983;Montagne et al, 2001;Peper, Bootsma, Mestre, & Bakker, 1994;Savelsbergh et al, 1991), it must be the case that the specific characteristics of this task require a prediction, as control of the action is not possible after releasing the ball.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This means that the temporal aspects of the movement did not have to be differently tailored to the individual trials; the same general pattern of movement initiation (or urgency) could work on all trials. Had the timing requirements been different from trial to trial, say on the basis of speed of arrival, then an explicit timing aspect might have proven necessary, as has been assumed by Peper et al (1994) and Dessing et al (2002).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given these doubts, Peper et al's conclusions about the general nature of Figure 1. Top and side views of the lateral interception task developed by Peper et al (1994) and used by Jacobs and Michaels (2006). A ball on a string swings down and passes to the right of the catcher.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This perspective shifts the attention from a perceptual account, based on the estimation of time-tocontact and the triggering of a preprogrammed movement with a fixed duration, to a more flexible dynamic account based on a continuous coupling of movement to information (e.g. Schöner 1994;Peper et al 1994;Dessing et al 2002). Of particular importance is that a continuously controlled action can be initiated on the basis of a critical value of an information source as well.…”
Section: Information Sourcesmentioning
confidence: 99%