2014
DOI: 10.5406/amerjpsyc.127.2.0157
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cardiovascular Change During Encoding Predicts the Nonconscious Mere Exposure Effect

Abstract: These studies examined memory encoding to determine whether the mere exposure effect could be categorized as a form of conceptual or perceptual implicit priming and, if it was not conceptual or perceptual, whether cardiovascular psychophysiology could reveal its nature. Experiment 1 examined the effects of study phase level of processing on recognition, the mere exposure effect, and word identification implicit priming. Deep relative to shallow processing improved recognition but did not influence the mere exp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
1
7
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The observation that the mere exposure effect was diminished, at approximately equivalent levels, by both trait and state anxiety is consistent with the observation that increased vasodilatation, as measured by blood volume pulse during encoding, predicts affective preference during retrieval ( Ladd et al, 2014 ). Vasodilatation is the relative reciprocal of heart rate ( Cowings et al, 2001 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The observation that the mere exposure effect was diminished, at approximately equivalent levels, by both trait and state anxiety is consistent with the observation that increased vasodilatation, as measured by blood volume pulse during encoding, predicts affective preference during retrieval ( Ladd et al, 2014 ). Vasodilatation is the relative reciprocal of heart rate ( Cowings et al, 2001 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Despite over four decades of extensive scientific inquiry spearheaded by his research, the psychological mechanism underlying the mere exposure effect remains uncertain (reviewed in Butler and Berry, 2004 ; Moreland and Topolinski, 2010 ). Two major explanatory frameworks for the mere exposure effect are (a) emotional or affective processing ( Zajonc, 1968 ; Kunst-Wilson and Zajonc, 1980 ; Zebrowitz and Zhang, 2012 ; Ladd et al, 2014 ), and (b) processing fluency or ease of processing ( Bornstein and D’Agostino, 1994 ; Seamon et al, 1995 ; Topolinski and Strack, 2009 ). Here we examined whether trait and state anxiety ( Spielberger et al, 1983 ), which have been associated with the experience of emotion ( Watson et al, 1988 ; Clark and Watson, 1991 ), independent from processing fluency, influence the mere exposure effect.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The ERP contrasts (old vs. new; clear vs. blurry) uncover novel evidence that can provide important insight into the mechanisms responsible for mere-exposure effects. For example, a similar pattern of ERP results for repetition (i.e., old vs. new) and clarity (i.e., clear vs. blurry) across the two different testing contexts of random and blocked clarity sequences would support accounts of bottom-up influences on affective judgments (e.g., Zajonc, 2001;Ladd et al, 2014). Such results would indicate that the perceptual qualities of the stimuli or implicit memory are driving the affective judgments.…”
Section: Study Predictionsmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…Manipulating perceptual fluency independently of stimulus repetitionand recording ERPshas the ability to adjudicate between the two conflicting theoretical accounts of mere exposure noted earlier. One explanation of mere-exposure effects contends that fluency is a bottom-up (e.g., data driven) process (e.g., Ladd et al, 2014;Zajonc, 2001) that leads subjects to endorse stimuli with more positive affective responses. Alternative accounts argue that mere exposure effects instead result from an interpretation of fluency that operates in a top-down fashion that considers stimulus features in context (e.g., Dechêne, Stahl, Hansen, & Wänke, 2009;Whittlesea & Price, 2001).…”
Section: Study Goalsmentioning
confidence: 99%