Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
2021
DOI: 10.1002/ajp.23311
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Captive great apes tend to innovate simple tool behaviors quickly

Abstract: Recent studies have highlighted the important role that individual learning mechanisms and different forms of enhancenment play in the acquisition of novel behaviors by naïve individuals. A considerable subset of these studies has focused on tool innovation by our closest living relatives, the great apes, to better undestand the evolution of technology in our own lineage. To be able to isolate the role that individual learning plays in great ape tool innovation, researchers usually employ what are known as bas… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 114 publications
(205 reference statements)
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, it is still under discussion whether non-human primates have enduring functional object representations (i.e., tools represented as being for specific purposes), as humans have (Vaesen, 2012;Ruiz and Santos, 2013). Although great apes show relatively high degrees of innovation and flexibility in their tool use (Manrique et al, 2013;Motes-Rodrigo and Tennie, 2022), they may get inflexibly fixated on familiar tool functions, resulting in a decrease in problem-solving performance when required to apply a new function to a given tool (Gruber, 2016;Ebel et al, 2021). Whether this fixedness is based on functional representations or sensorimotor processes (and then it would rather fall under means-end knowledge, see next section) need to be examined more closely as well as the interaction of both processes (flexibility and fixation), also considering similar phenomena in human children's tool use (German and Defeyter, 2000;Defeyter and German, 2003).…”
Section: Functional Tool Knowledgementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, it is still under discussion whether non-human primates have enduring functional object representations (i.e., tools represented as being for specific purposes), as humans have (Vaesen, 2012;Ruiz and Santos, 2013). Although great apes show relatively high degrees of innovation and flexibility in their tool use (Manrique et al, 2013;Motes-Rodrigo and Tennie, 2022), they may get inflexibly fixated on familiar tool functions, resulting in a decrease in problem-solving performance when required to apply a new function to a given tool (Gruber, 2016;Ebel et al, 2021). Whether this fixedness is based on functional representations or sensorimotor processes (and then it would rather fall under means-end knowledge, see next section) need to be examined more closely as well as the interaction of both processes (flexibility and fixation), also considering similar phenomena in human children's tool use (German and Defeyter, 2000;Defeyter and German, 2003).…”
Section: Functional Tool Knowledgementioning
confidence: 99%
“…This over‐representation of apes reflects patterns identified in previous reviews of zoo‐based primate research (Cronin et al, 2017; Hopper, 2017; Lewis & Krupenye, 2022; Melfi, 2005, 2007; Stoinski et al, 1998), but does not reflect the population of primates living in zoos: only 15% of the primates housed in North American AZA‐accredited zoos are great apes ( Gorilla, Pan , and Pongo ) (Species360, 2021). Accordingly, for this special issue I wanted to invite contributions from researchers studying zoo‐housed monkeys and prosimians (e.g., Cairo‐Evans et al, 2022; Hayden et al, 2022; Spiezio et al, 2022) in addition to those studying apes (e.g., Laméris et al, 2022; Martin & Shumaker, 2022; Motes‐Rodrigo & Tennie, 2022; Vonk, 2022; Yamanashi et al, 2022).…”
Section: Genus Number Of Projects Percentage Of Projectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Accordingly, the contributed articles in this special issue also reflect the growing use of technology to help us monitor, evaluate, and enhance primate welfare (e.g., Cairo‐Evans et al, 2022; Hayden et al, 2022; Laméris et al, 2022; Vonk, 2022; Yamanashi et al, 2022). Moreover, technological advances also offer refinements to the ways in which we can study primate behavior and cognition in zoos, via the use of touchscreens (Martin & Shumaker, 2022), eye‐tracking devices (Lewis & Krupenye, 2022), and automated behavioral monitoring systems (Hayden et al, 2022), although “physical” tasks also continue to be used to test zoo primate cognition (Motes‐Rodrigo & Tennie, 2022; Spiezio et al, 2022), as reviewed in McEwen et al ( 2022). These technological approaches offer more nuanced perspectives about the experiences of primates while also allowing for more flexible testing procedures that promote primate welfare (Hopper et al, 2021; Lewis & Krupenye, 2022; Ross et al in press).…”
Section: Genus Number Of Projects Percentage Of Projectsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This could explain why chimpanzees, for example, reportedly engage in problem-solving activities even when no reward is involved (Clark and Smith 2013 ). Furthermore, great apes often explore novel objects (Paquette and Prescott 1988 ), possess highly developed manipulative skills (Paquette and Prescott 1988 ; Torigoe 1985 ), and use and create tools in captive environments (Motes-Rodrigo and Tennie 2021 ). Therefore, the introduction of novel devices or tasks promoting such behaviors may be a particularly successful enrichment strategy for these species.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%