2005
DOI: 10.1017/s0272263105050114
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

CAN SECOND LANGUAGE GRAMMAR BE LEARNED THROUGH LISTENING?: An Experimental Study

Abstract: This study examines whether aural processing of input in a situation of implicit instruction can build a knowledge base that is available for both comprehension and production tasks. Fifty-five Dutch students learned a miniature linguistic system based on Spanish. Three training conditions were compared in which noun-adjective gender agreement was the learning target. The first group of participants received receptive training, the second group received receptive and productive training, and a third group serv… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

6
38
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
6
38
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Even for learners at advanced levels of proficiency, errors in gender agreement appear to persist (Dewaele & Veronique, 2001; Franceschina, 2005). Although this issue has been addressed from multiple perspectives (Arteaga, Herschensohn, & Gess, 2003; Bartning, 2000; Benati, 2005; De Jong, 2005; Dewaele & Veronique, 2001; Franceschina, 2005; Gass & Alvarez Torres, 2005; Hawkins & Chan, 1997; Keating, 2009; Montrul, 2004; Montrul et al, 2008; White, Valenzuela, Kozlowska‐Macgregor, & Leung, 2004), neurocognitive research has only begun to consider the development, representation, and processing of L2 grammatical gender (Sabourin & Haverkort, 2003; Sabourin & Stowe, 2008; Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005). The current article aims to contribute to the neurocognitive evidence pertaining to the L2 acquisition and processing of grammatical gender by examining the online processing of noun‐phrase gender agreement on both articles and adjectives as affected by two different types of language training conditions—explicit (classroomlike) and implicit (immersionlike)—at both low and high proficiency.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even for learners at advanced levels of proficiency, errors in gender agreement appear to persist (Dewaele & Veronique, 2001; Franceschina, 2005). Although this issue has been addressed from multiple perspectives (Arteaga, Herschensohn, & Gess, 2003; Bartning, 2000; Benati, 2005; De Jong, 2005; Dewaele & Veronique, 2001; Franceschina, 2005; Gass & Alvarez Torres, 2005; Hawkins & Chan, 1997; Keating, 2009; Montrul, 2004; Montrul et al, 2008; White, Valenzuela, Kozlowska‐Macgregor, & Leung, 2004), neurocognitive research has only begun to consider the development, representation, and processing of L2 grammatical gender (Sabourin & Haverkort, 2003; Sabourin & Stowe, 2008; Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005). The current article aims to contribute to the neurocognitive evidence pertaining to the L2 acquisition and processing of grammatical gender by examining the online processing of noun‐phrase gender agreement on both articles and adjectives as affected by two different types of language training conditions—explicit (classroomlike) and implicit (immersionlike)—at both low and high proficiency.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dekeyser concluded that declarative knowledge could be converted into an automatic one through proceduralization. Similarly, this end is supported by a study conducted by de Jong (2005) who proposed that the more aural comprehension his Spanish learners had the more speed they had gained in comprehension. But this stands in contrast to errors they made in terms of language production.…”
Section: Introduction and Literature Reviewmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…Jong (2005) claims that production task requires more cognitive demands than comprehension task. SP tasks require the subjects to invest more effort and perform numerous skills of generating ideas, selecting vocabularies, structuring sentences and deciding on the correct use of the verb "be".…”
Section: Sentence Production and Grammaticality Judgmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This can be attributed to the design of SP tasks that Group 1 (SP) was assigned with during the study. SP tasks are considered as production tasks that have higher demand than comprehension tasks (Jong, 2005) which leads them to pay more attention to the target form (Schmidt, 1990). Hence, learners in Group 1 (SP) were more aware of the correct form of SVA due to the level of attention they had devoted to that particular feature.…”
Section: Students' Perception Of the Benefits Of Cr Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation