2008
DOI: 10.1037/1076-898x.14.2.192
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Camera perspective bias in videotaped confessions: Evidence that visual attention is a mediator.

Abstract: Several experiments have demonstrated a camera perspective bias in evaluations of videotaped confessions: videotapes with the camera focused on the suspect lead to judgments of greater voluntariness than alternative presentation formats. The present research investigated potential mediators of this bias. Using eye tracking to measure visual attention, Experiment 1 replicated the bias and revealed that changes in camera perspective are accompanied by corresponding changes in duration of fixation on the suspect … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
31
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As explained earlier, this effect may occur because (a) observers visually attend to the more salient individual within the scene and also judge that person as having a more causal role (Lassiter, Beers et al, 2002;Taylor & Fiske, 1978) and (b) observers use visual content within the video to assess voluntariness, and content varies with camera perspective (Ware et al, 2008). Thus, when the focus was on the defendant, our jurors assigned him more responsibility for choosing to confess, which in turn made his statement seem more likely authentic and more incriminating.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As explained earlier, this effect may occur because (a) observers visually attend to the more salient individual within the scene and also judge that person as having a more causal role (Lassiter, Beers et al, 2002;Taylor & Fiske, 1978) and (b) observers use visual content within the video to assess voluntariness, and content varies with camera perspective (Ware et al, 2008). Thus, when the focus was on the defendant, our jurors assigned him more responsibility for choosing to confess, which in turn made his statement seem more likely authentic and more incriminating.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…This effect may be caused by two factors (Ware, Lassiter, Patterson, & Ransom, 2008). First, observers tend to direct their visual attention toward the individual on whom the camera is focused and to 'overestimate the causal role of the individual who is most visually prominent' (Lassiter, Beers et al, 2002, p. 267), as suggested by social attribution research (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…While the current experiment does not allow for a direct test of the positive or negative consequence of priming, converging evidence from other sources suggests that this attentional bias is likely negative. Attention to Black faces has been linked with threat perception [24] and, in legal settings, similar attention effects have promoted perceptions of guilt [25]. Indeed, activating the social category Black has been demonstrated to have negative consequences on legal judgments [12].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Geller, 1992). Equal-focus videotapes yield less biased evaluations because neither the suspect nor interrogator is more visually conspicuous, and therefore neither one draws a disproportionate amount of observers' visual attention (Lassiter et al, 2002c;Ware, Lassiter, Patterson, & Ransom, 2008). An alternative method of making the suspect and interrogator equally prominent in observers' visual fields is to record each one with a separate camera positioned so that face-on views can be captured of both.…”
Section: The Camera Perspective Bias In Videotaped Interrogations-conmentioning
confidence: 99%