1994
DOI: 10.1080/00207149408409352
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Brain Dynamics and Hypnosis: Attentional and Disattentional Processes

Abstract: This article reviews recent research findings, expanding an evolving neuropsychophysiological model of hypnosis (Crawford, 1989; Crawford & Gruzelier, 1992), that support the view that highly hypnotizable persons (highs) possess stronger attentional filtering abilities than do low hypnotizable persons, and that these differences are reflected in underlying brain dynamics. Behavioral, cognitive, and neurophysiological evidence is reviewed that suggests that highs can both better focus and sustain their attentio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

7
82
0
2

Year Published

1998
1998
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 170 publications
(92 citation statements)
references
References 103 publications
(108 reference statements)
7
82
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Evans and Graham went on to show that random number generation was a facility associated with high hypnotic susceptibility (Evans and Graham, 1980). Subsequently Crawford and Gruzelier and colleagues (Malott et al, 1989;Crawford and Gruzelier, 1992;Crawford, 1994;Gruzelier, 1998) used this concept as an explanatory construct for cognitive and neurophysiological findings that have differentiated high from low hypnotic susceptibility. Among these were responsiveness to visual illusions and reversible figures, susceptibility to autokinetic movement, stronger attentional filtering abilities, the ability to shift from analytic to holistic strategies and the ability to display requisite hemispheric specificity in alignment with task demands.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Evans and Graham went on to show that random number generation was a facility associated with high hypnotic susceptibility (Evans and Graham, 1980). Subsequently Crawford and Gruzelier and colleagues (Malott et al, 1989;Crawford and Gruzelier, 1992;Crawford, 1994;Gruzelier, 1998) used this concept as an explanatory construct for cognitive and neurophysiological findings that have differentiated high from low hypnotic susceptibility. Among these were responsiveness to visual illusions and reversible figures, susceptibility to autokinetic movement, stronger attentional filtering abilities, the ability to shift from analytic to holistic strategies and the ability to display requisite hemispheric specificity in alignment with task demands.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…It is a multidimensional characteristic including various abilities, i.e. imagery (Crawford, 1982;Glisky, Tataryn and Kihlstrom, 1995;Lyons and Crawford, 1997;Kogon, Jasiukaitis, Berardi, Gupta, Kosslyn and Spiegel, 1998), fantasy-proneness (Wilson and Barber, 1982;Lynn and Ruhe, 1986), expectancy (Council, Kirsch and Hafner, 1986), attention/absorption (Tellegen and Atkins, 1974;Crawford, 1989;1994;Crawford, Brown and Moon, 1993), acquiescence and consistency motivation (Council and Green, 2004). Imagery, particularly in the visual modality, has been frequently considered to be mainly responsible for hypnotizability (Glisky et al, 1995).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The ability to focus attention on the hypnotist's voice and ignore irrelevant stimuli is required to complete the fi rst stage of hypnosis as described by the neuropsychophysiological model of hypnotic induction (Crawford and Gruzelier, 1992;Crawford, 1994;Gruzelier, 1998;2006). Highly hypnotizable subjects should, therefore, show a greater performance of attentional functioning both in and out of hypnosis.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%