2005
DOI: 10.1207/s1532799xssr0901_3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Brain Activity During Performance of Naming Tasks: Comparison Between Dyslexic and Regular Readers

Abstract: This research was aimed at contributing to the current understanding of the underlying factors of naming speed and the causes of naming speed deficits. Forty regular readers and 40 dyslexic university students participated in the study. Electrophysiological (Event-Related Potentials [ERPs]) and behavioral measures were employed. Behavioral baseline tasks assessed general ability, reading skills, reading-related cognitive abilities, and standard Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) and Rapid Alternating Stimulus test… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
11
0
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
(62 reference statements)
1
11
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, working with preschool children with and without familial risk of reading difficulties, Raschle and colleagues (2011) reported a significant relationship of the left temporoparietal and occipitotemporal regions with RAN such that, as grey matter volume increased, RAN performance improved (see also Eckert, 2004, for a review of structural findings in individuals with dyslexia). Overall, the shared RAN-reading brain regions identified here are in line with previous hypotheses regarding the neural correlates of RAN and our findings are consistent with the current reading literature (see Breznitz, 2005;Guenther & Vladusich, 2012;Indefrey & Levelt, 2004;Misra et al, 2004;Price, 2012).…”
Section: Whole Brain Mean Activation Mapssupporting
confidence: 92%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For example, working with preschool children with and without familial risk of reading difficulties, Raschle and colleagues (2011) reported a significant relationship of the left temporoparietal and occipitotemporal regions with RAN such that, as grey matter volume increased, RAN performance improved (see also Eckert, 2004, for a review of structural findings in individuals with dyslexia). Overall, the shared RAN-reading brain regions identified here are in line with previous hypotheses regarding the neural correlates of RAN and our findings are consistent with the current reading literature (see Breznitz, 2005;Guenther & Vladusich, 2012;Indefrey & Levelt, 2004;Misra et al, 2004;Price, 2012).…”
Section: Whole Brain Mean Activation Mapssupporting
confidence: 92%
“…RAN has been found to be highly predictive of reading ability in multiple populations and across age groups (Breznitz, 2005;Kirby et al, 2003;Misra et al, 2004;Moll et al, 2009;Powell et al, 2007;Wile & Borowsky, 2004). We provided direct evidence that RAN and reading rely on highly similar neural regions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 60%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Electrophysiological recording has a high temporal precision that these measures lack, thereby providing very useful in any attempt to decompose the distinct cognitive processing stages required for production. Somewhat surprisingly, however, very few studies have taken advantage of these measures in the study of impaired visual naming in dyslexia (for an exception see Breznitz (2005) and ). As an example, using a silent object-and letter-naming task, Breznitz (2005) observed that dyslexics' slowness in naming is manifested already at the early stage of stimulus identification and classification (N1-P2-N2 complex), which persists when processing the information in memory (P300) and into the output stage.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Somewhat surprisingly, however, very few studies have taken advantage of these measures in the study of impaired visual naming in dyslexia (for an exception see Breznitz (2005) and ). As an example, using a silent object-and letter-naming task, Breznitz (2005) observed that dyslexics' slowness in naming is manifested already at the early stage of stimulus identification and classification (N1-P2-N2 complex), which persists when processing the information in memory (P300) and into the output stage. Maylseless and Breznitz (2011), in turn, found differences between groups when viewing objects and pseudo-objects in an object decision task, corresponding to the P1 and N1 components, i.e., shorter peak latencies were exhibited by the dyslexics, which is interpreted as due to the greater cognitive demands of the task on this group.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%