2008
DOI: 10.3758/brm.40.4.1075
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Body—object interaction ratings for 1,618 monosyllabic nouns

Abstract: The importance of sensorimotor interactions with the environment to the development and structure of cognitive processes is now being taken seriously by an increasing number of cognitive scientists. An embodied cognition approach to the study of cognitive processing has been adopted by researchers examining such varied abilities as cognitive and motor development, mental imagery, memory, reasoning and problem solving, linguistic processing, metaphor processing, and concept formation (Clark, 1997;Gibbs, 2006;Gl… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
116
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 96 publications
(125 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
6
116
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The correlation between SERs and imageability is higher in the combined data set (r 0 .586) than is reported in Juhasz et al (2011) for only monosyllabic words. The size of the correlation is similar to that reported between AoA and imageability (r 0 -.586) for the words presented in the Gilhooly and Logie (1980) norms (as calculated by Zevin & Seidenberg, 2002), as well as between BOI and imageability for 1,618 monosyllabic nouns (r 0 .67; Tillotson, Siakaluk, & Pexman, 2008). These correlations are smaller than those reported for different imageability norms (all of which have rs>.80; see Fugett, 2004, andSchock et al, 2012).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 38%
“…The correlation between SERs and imageability is higher in the combined data set (r 0 .586) than is reported in Juhasz et al (2011) for only monosyllabic words. The size of the correlation is similar to that reported between AoA and imageability (r 0 -.586) for the words presented in the Gilhooly and Logie (1980) norms (as calculated by Zevin & Seidenberg, 2002), as well as between BOI and imageability for 1,618 monosyllabic nouns (r 0 .67; Tillotson, Siakaluk, & Pexman, 2008). These correlations are smaller than those reported for different imageability norms (all of which have rs>.80; see Fugett, 2004, andSchock et al, 2012).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 38%
“…We therefore examined the correlations of valence and arousal with a battery of semantic measures, and we tested whether these emotional factors explained any unique variance in word recognition times after statistically accounting for those semantic variables. For this analysis we identified a set of 1083 monosyllabic words for which all of the following measures were available: valence and arousal ratings (Warriner et al, 2013), SUBTLEX frequency of occurrence (Brysbaert & New, 2009), age-of-acquisition ratings (Kuperman et al, 2012), imageability ratings (Cortese & Fugett, 2004; Schock et al, 2012), sensory experience ratings (Juhasz, Yap, Dicke, Taylor, & Gullick, 2011; Juhasz & Yap, 2013), body-object interaction ratings (Tillotson, Siakaluk, & Pexman, 2008), semantic diversity measures (Hoffman, Ralph, & Rogers, 2012; see also Jones, Johns, & Recchia, 2012) and the word's number of senses from Wordnet (Miller, 1995). …”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The reliabilities of the norms are high and nearly identical. Table 2 shows the correlation coefficients between the present norms and those obtained for (1) subjective frequency in Chinese (Liu et al, 2011), in American English (Balota et al, 2001), and in French (Bonin, Méot, et al, 2003); (2) image variability in Chinese (Liu et al, 2011), in French (Alario & Ferrand, 1999), and in Spanish (Sanfeliu & Fernandez, 1996); and (3) BOI for Canadian English (Tillotson, Siakaluk, & Pexman, 2008). Given that these norms were all developed for different purposes, they were not computed using the same items.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%