2009
DOI: 10.1167/9.1.10
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Binocular depth discrimination and estimation beyond interaction space

Abstract: The benefits of binocular vision have been debated throughout the history of vision science yet few studies have considered its contribution beyond a viewing distance of a few meters. In the first set of experiments, we compared monocular and binocular performance on depth interval estimation and discrimination tasks at 4.5, 9.0 or 18.0 m. Under monocular conditions, perceived depth was significantly compressed. Binocular depth estimates were much nearer to veridical although also compressed. Regression-based … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
62
1
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 84 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
1
62
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The current study extended and developed the work of Allison et al (2009), utilizing a similar experimental setup. However, we examined the stereoscopic perceptions of a much wider range of depths (from 0 to 248 m), which were presented at larger observation distances (20 or 40 m).…”
Section: Previous Studiesmentioning
confidence: 90%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The current study extended and developed the work of Allison et al (2009), utilizing a similar experimental setup. However, we examined the stereoscopic perceptions of a much wider range of depths (from 0 to 248 m), which were presented at larger observation distances (20 or 40 m).…”
Section: Previous Studiesmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…In a more recent study by Allison, Gillam, and Vecellio (2009), subjects were binocularly and monocularly exposed to pairs of light emitting diode (LED) targets, either in complete darkness or when the foreground of the laboratory was lit (i.e., up to the nearer of the two LED targets). The region between the two LEDs was always dark and provided only binocular disparity information about depth.…”
Section: Previous Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, this observation is in contrast with the apparent depth between objects in a three-dimensional scene, which is reduced for monocular rather than binocular viewing (Allison, Gillam, & Vecellio, 2009;Foley, 1980;Frisby, Buckley, & Duke, 1996;Gogel, 1962;Grant, 1942;Koenderink, van Doorn, & Kappers, 1995;Komoda & Ono, 1974;McKee & Taylor, 2010;Swenson, 1932). Schlosberg (1941) explained augmentation of the plastic effect in monocular conditions by the reduction of "flatness" cues.…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…In addition, we believe that further experimentation with the baselines of the cameras might be interesting. Human baseline is limited to 19 m (Allison et al, 2009), but with camera capturing systems, this can be modified and enable greater distance estimations when necessary. Our system had also issues that would make it hard to operate in a real environment, such as the weight of the setup, 3.3 kg with batteries, which significantly reduces the remaining payload capacity of the drone and the latency of 250 ms, which makes it impossible to fly the drone at speeds higher than 7 m/s.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The TPV type of operation creates control and safety problems, since operators have difficulty guessing the drone orientation and may not be able to keep eye contact with them even at short distances (>100 m). Furthermore, given that effective human stereo vision is limited to 19 m (Allison et al, 2009), it is hard for humans to estimate drone's position relative to nearby objects to avoid obstacles and control attitude/altitude. In such cases, the ground control gets more challenging the further the drone gets from the operator.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%