1988
DOI: 10.1121/1.396629
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Binaural summation and lateralization of transients: A combined analysis

Abstract: Subjects judged the loudness and the lateral position of dichotic transient signals, which were presented at equal and unequal levels, synchronously and asynchronously, to the two ears. Binaural loudness summation of clicks does not obey a law of linear addition: It is partial at low level and superadditive at high level. Supersummation is greater for interaurally delayed clicks than for coincidental ones. The relation between click loudness and sound pressure (over moderate SLs) can be described as a power fu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
2

Year Published

1989
1989
2006
2006

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
(62 reference statements)
1
10
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather, there is a nonsignificant trend in the data for the diotic stimuli to be perceived as quieter than the dichotic stimuli, as might be predicted from the literature (e.g., Algom, Adam, & Cohen-Raz, 1988;Algom & Marks, 1984;Reynolds & Stevens, 1960). Thus, the perceived intensity change observed in Experiment 2 is special to the ILLU stimulus, since it was not seen when single, isolated tones were presented instead of a sequence (Experiment 3) or when the high and low tones did not alternate between ears (Experiment 4).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 54%
“…Rather, there is a nonsignificant trend in the data for the diotic stimuli to be perceived as quieter than the dichotic stimuli, as might be predicted from the literature (e.g., Algom, Adam, & Cohen-Raz, 1988;Algom & Marks, 1984;Reynolds & Stevens, 1960). Thus, the perceived intensity change observed in Experiment 2 is special to the ILLU stimulus, since it was not seen when single, isolated tones were presented instead of a sequence (Experiment 3) or when the high and low tones did not alternate between ears (Experiment 4).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 54%
“…The results of Irvin (1965), Marks (1980), Reynolds and S. S. Stevens (1960), Scharf (1968), Scharf and Fishken (1970), and recently, of Algom et al (1988) showed a similar change with level for noise stimuli. At low levels, the monaural noise had to be about 4 dB greater than the binaural noise to be judged as loud, but this difference increased to about 9 dB, in the present set of short noise bursts, at levels around 70 dB.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 59%
“…For wide-band noise stimuli, on the other hand, the binaural-monaural ratio increases with increasing sound pressure (Algom et al, 1988;Irwin, 1965;Marks, 1980Marks, , 1987Reynolds & S. S. Stevens, 1960;Scharf, 1968;Scharf & Fishken, 1970). Although Reynolds and Stevens (1960) concluded that the loudness of both monaural and binaural noise stimuli grows as a power function of sound pressure (but that the exponents differ), Scharf and Fishken (1970) and Marks (1980), as well as the majority of other investigations (see Scharf, 1978, for a review), have reported psychophysical functions for noise that are not power functions of sound pressure.…”
Section: Binaural Summation Of Tones and Noisesmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The basic features characterizing binaural, dichotic, subcritical, and supercritical loudness integration have been documented (e.g., by Algom, Adam, & Cohen-Raz, 1988;Algom & Marks, 1984;Marks, 1978aMarks, , 1978bMarks, , 1979aMarks, , 1979bMarks, , 1980Scharf, 1970Scharf, , 1978. Loudness shows (1) a superiority of binaural to monaural listening, (2) invariance within a critical band, but (3) dependence on frequency separation beyond the critical band (diotic stimulation).…”
Section: Mode Of Presentationmentioning
confidence: 99%