2018
DOI: 10.1177/0956797617742725
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Beyond the 30-Million-Word Gap: Children’s Conversational Exposure Is Associated With Language-Related Brain Function

Abstract: Children's early language exposure impacts their later linguistic skills, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement, and large disparities in language exposure are associated with family socioeconomic status (SES). However, there is little evidence about the neural mechanisms underlying the relation between language experience and linguistic and cognitive development. Here, language experience was measured from home audio recordings of 36 SES-diverse 4- to 6-year-old children. During a story-listening func… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

36
392
3
14

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 504 publications
(450 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
36
392
3
14
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, Romeo et al. (), Hutton et al. (), and numerous behavioral studies (e.g., Cartmill et al., ; Malin et al., ) find that language quality and reading quality are contributors to children's brain development and language growth.…”
Section: Quantity Versus Quality Of Communicative Exchanges: What Reamentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Thus, Romeo et al. (), Hutton et al. (), and numerous behavioral studies (e.g., Cartmill et al., ; Malin et al., ) find that language quality and reading quality are contributors to children's brain development and language growth.…”
Section: Quantity Versus Quality Of Communicative Exchanges: What Reamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, language input that meaningfully builds on what children are doing in the moment, is rooted in shared attention, and models the use of syntax by using recasts and expansions of their utterances best facilitates language learning (e.g., Harris, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2010;Malin, Cabrera, & Rowe, 2014;McGillion et al, 2017). As countless studies have shown, back-and-forth conversations that are both temporally and topically contingent on children's contribution, are the fuel that prime the learning of language (Goldin-Meadow et al, 2014;Reed, Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2017;Romeo et al, 2018;Tamis-LeMonda, Kuchirko, & Song, 2014). Second, the social-cognitive demands of overheard speech likewise exceed those for child-directed speech in that children have to infer others' interests and intentions, making sense of third-party interactions rather than one-on-one exchanges.…”
Section: Speech Directed To Children Supports Language Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Echoing these behavioral findings, in a recent study using fMRI, less advantaged parents had fewer conversational exchanges with their 4‐ to 6‐year‐olds than more advantaged parents. In turn, children who experienced fewer conversational exchanges had reduced activation in left inferior frontal regions during language processing (Romeo et al., ).…”
Section: Mediators Linking Socioeconomic Background With Language Andmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, the amount of infant-directed speech and turn-taking that children experience predicts their speech and language development (e.g. Ramírez-Esparza, García-Sierra, & Kuhl, 2014;Romeo et al, 2018). Another source of variability is the number of languages that they hear.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%