Cognitive Biases in Health and Psychiatric Disorders 2020
DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-816660-4.00001-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Beyond negativity: Motivational relevance as cause of attentional bias to positive stimuli

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 90 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Altogether, the uncertainty of a neutral infant face reflects multiphase emotional appraisals for ambiguous valence on highly goal-relevant stimuli (Vogt et al, 2019). These appraisals ultimately allow the individual to enter a state in which it is easier to recognise the infant (Ding et al, 2016; Jia et al, 2017) and to become more refined in the operation (Nittono et al, 2012), which lays the foundation for the provision of good parenting.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Altogether, the uncertainty of a neutral infant face reflects multiphase emotional appraisals for ambiguous valence on highly goal-relevant stimuli (Vogt et al, 2019). These appraisals ultimately allow the individual to enter a state in which it is easier to recognise the infant (Ding et al, 2016; Jia et al, 2017) and to become more refined in the operation (Nittono et al, 2012), which lays the foundation for the provision of good parenting.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The current review focuses on well-known biases that reflect distorted processing in health as well as in psychopathology: (1) expectancy bias, in which individuals overestimate the likelihood of encountering the fear-relevant stimulus (encounter bias) or the negative outcome that will follow the encounter [consequence bias; for reviews on negative and positive expectancy bias, see (24,25), respectively]; (2) attention bias, which is exhibited through faster engagement with and slower disengagement from the fear-relevant stimulus, followed by attentional avoidance of said stimuli [for reviews on negative and positive attention bias, see (26,27), respectively]; (3) memory bias, in which individuals remember fear-related items more often than fearunrelated items [for reviews on negative and positive memory bias, see (28,29), respectively]; (4) perception bias, in which individuals overestimate a physical characteristic of the fearrelevant stimulus, such as its size or distance [e.g., (30)(31)(32); for a recent review, see (33)]; (5) interpretation bias, in which individuals interpret ambiguous stimuli as threatening [for reviews on negative and positive interpretation biases, see (34,35), respectively]. Although these biases exist in healthy populations, they are more severe and persistent in populations with psychopathologies [for a review, see (36)].…”
Section: The Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given the immediate existential jeopardy associated with threatening stimuli, it could be argued that these should always be prioritized over self-cues in the attention system. Indeed, some authors argue for a general attention bias for negative over positive stimuli (Ito et al, 1998; Öhman & Mineka, 2001; Pratto & John, 1991; although see Becker et al, 2011; Vogt et al, 2020). Since threat cues are inherently negative while self is considered a positive stimulus (Beggan, 1992; Robins & Beer, 2001), this would indicate that threatening cues should be positioned higher than self-cues in the attentional hierarchy.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%