1996
DOI: 10.3758/bf03197277
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Beyond category sorting and pleasantness rating: Inducing relational and item-specific processing

Abstract: In most studies of relational and item-specific processing, category sorting and pleasantness rating have been the main procedures used to induce these two types of processing. Because the two types of processing have been studied in a wide range of memory phenomena (Hunt & McDaniel, 1993), it is strange that other tasks have not been proposed and tested. The present experiment demonstrates that equivalent results can be obtained with three relational processing tasks (category sorting, narrative construction,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

2
20
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
2
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition to the control group, which was given the standard memory instructions, a second group was required to form an itemspecific image of each list item, and a third group was instructed to form relational images of list items interacting. Previous research has demonstrated that the item-specific imagery task promotes the encoding of item-specific information, whereas the relational imagery task leads to relational processing of the list items (see, e.g., Burns & Gold, 1999;Hodge & Otani, 1996).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to the control group, which was given the standard memory instructions, a second group was required to form an itemspecific image of each list item, and a third group was instructed to form relational images of list items interacting. Previous research has demonstrated that the item-specific imagery task promotes the encoding of item-specific information, whereas the relational imagery task leads to relational processing of the list items (see, e.g., Burns & Gold, 1999;Hodge & Otani, 1996).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Experiment 1B used weakly related lists in which study items were from broad, unspecified categories. Based on prior findings (Engelkamp, Biegelmann, & McDaniel, 1998; Hodge & Otani, 1996; Hunt & Einstein, 1981; McDaniel, Einstein, et al, 1988), memory was expected to be better with under complimentary (vs. redundant) processing conditions. In this way, complimentary processing was expected to serve as a type of EV in which the processing afforded by the study task and list structure do not overlap.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In Experiment 1, participants studied the same set of items across two study blocks. In the first block, item-specific processing was induced either through mental-imagery (MI) or pleasantness-rating (PR) tasks, or relational processing was induced either through category-sorting (CS) or narrative-construction (NC) tasks (after Hodge & Otani, 1996). On the second block, the same words were studied again using either the same task as the first block ( repeated-task groups ), a different task that required the same type of processing ( variable-task group ), or a different task that required the other type of processing ( variable-processing groups ; see Table 1).…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This two-process explanation seems particularly useful for explaining the results of experiments that used non-scenario control conditions (e.g., pleasantness rating, imagery rating, generation), because many of these control conditions are known to induce primarily item-specific processing (e.g., Burns, Curti & Lavin, 1993;Einstein & Hunt, 1980;Hodge & Otani, 1996). It is not clear, however, why survival processing would produce more item-specific or more relational processing than control conditions involving other scenarios (e.g., moving, robbery, etc.).…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%