1998
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1998.tb02684.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Believing details known to have been suggested

Abstract: In two experiments, participants viewed a videotape of a simulated armed robbery, later answered misleading questions about it, and then finally completed a source monitoring test. For the test, participants were asked to indicate for each test item whether it was (1) seen in the video only, (2) read about in the questions only, (3) both seen and read about, (4) not remembered or (5) known to have occurred but the source was unclear. The latter response category was included on the test to remove source guessi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
65
0
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 78 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
9
65
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The rate of misinformation endorsement was consistent with and even slightly higher than in previous studies (Cann & Katz, 2005;Tomes & Katz, 1997), possibly because of the additional events and the longer interval between encoding and testing (Higham, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 65%
“…The rate of misinformation endorsement was consistent with and even slightly higher than in previous studies (Cann & Katz, 2005;Tomes & Katz, 1997), possibly because of the additional events and the longer interval between encoding and testing (Higham, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 65%
“…In some studies, the influence persisted despite postwarnings (Belli, Lindsay, Gales, & McCarthy, 1994;Frost, Ingraham, & Wilson, 2002;Greene et al, 1982;Higham, 1998;Zaragoza & Lane, 1994, Experiment 4), whereas in others it was reduced (Chambers & Zaragoza, 2001;Highhouse & Bottrill, 1995;Meade & Roediger, 2002) or even eliminated by postwarning manipulations (Blank, 1998;Wright, 1993;Zaragoza & Koshmider, 1989). In research reported by Lindsay (1990), a postwarning diminished the misinformation effect only when the event and the postevent source shared many characteristics, not when they shared only a few characteristics.…”
Section: Pre-versus Postwarnings: Manipulations Before and After The mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…In most studies, the postwarning took the form of an explicit instruction alerting participants more or less directly to discrepancies between the witnessed event and the postevent information. Apparently, postwarnings that are rather unspecific (Belli et al, 1994;Christiaansen & Ochalek, 1983;Frost et al, 2002;Zaragoza & Lane, 1994) or are phrased in the subjunctive mood (Greene et al, 1982;Higham, 1998) are less effective than postwarnings that are specific concerning misleading details (Wright, 1993) or that are spelled out in the indicative mood (e.g., Lindsay, 1990), thus pointing out definite discrepancies between event and postevent information. 1 Furthermore, eyewitnesses are apparently less likely to report misleading items if there is an additional interpersonal or social incentive to resist the influence of misinformation (see Chambers & Zaragoza, 2001).…”
Section: Pre-versus Postwarnings: Manipulations Before and After The mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Next to each number were a yes/no response and five alternative source response options (i.e., saw, read, both, don't know, and neither). Higham (1998) suggested this five-source option in order to rule out the possibility of source guessing arising from familiarity misjudgments. The meaning of the first three of these options is self-evident.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%