2018
DOI: 10.1080/1751696x.2018.1469230
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

‘Behavioral modernity’ as a process, not an event, in the human niche

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
30
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 99 publications
0
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Behavioural and cognitive modernity are thought to have come about between 100 000 and 30 000 years ago, as evidenced by the proliferation in cultural artefacts of both utilitarian and aesthetic value. (Although some researchers argue that the onset of behavioural modernity was less pronounced than once thought [1][2][3], and the concept of behavioural modernity itself has been called into question [4], this paper does not delve into these discussions so as to focus squarely on the task of modelling the cognitive changes underlying this cultural transition.) Some attribute this transition to an enhanced ability to process social information [5,6].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Behavioural and cognitive modernity are thought to have come about between 100 000 and 30 000 years ago, as evidenced by the proliferation in cultural artefacts of both utilitarian and aesthetic value. (Although some researchers argue that the onset of behavioural modernity was less pronounced than once thought [1][2][3], and the concept of behavioural modernity itself has been called into question [4], this paper does not delve into these discussions so as to focus squarely on the task of modelling the cognitive changes underlying this cultural transition.) Some attribute this transition to an enhanced ability to process social information [5,6].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Although cultural transmission-in which one individual acquires elements of culture from another-is observed in many species, cultural evolution is much rarer, and perhaps unique to our species. 1 Critical to cultural evolution is the capacity to combine ideas, adapt existing solutions to new situations and reframe information in one's own terms. 2 This paper uses network theory to address how this capacity arose.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The package of morphological traits we once labeled “anatomically modern” shows up in varying forms, in varying places, and in different populations of Homo across Africa and Eurasia over the last 400,000–300,000 years 7 . Almost every type of material evidence, of “symbol” or other indicators of substantive and repetitive meaning‐making, 45 that were considered indicators of uniquely modern human cognition also show up associated with populations that do not have contemporary human morphology 42 . It has become increasingly clear that what makes us human is not the emergence of a chin, more vertical frontal bones or a high rounded cranium, nor is it the creation of symbolic artifacts, language, conscious thought, or anything of that nature.…”
Section: What Is a Human?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For the last half century paleoanthropologists and archeologists relied on two major developments as having "identified" the emergence of contemporary humans: behavioral modernity and anatomical modernity. 7,8,[40][41][42] Anatomical modernity (fossils that are morphologically contiguous with contemporary humans) was initially used as a core marker of "being human." But the dates of such fossils are highly variable, going from 100,000 years ago, to 180,000 years ago, to 300,000 years ago, depending on which fossils and which morphological markers one uses.…”
Section: What Is a Human?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Modern human behaviour (defined as behaviours that indicate modern-level linguistic and cognitive abilities and identified archaeologically through the presence of, among others, deliberate burials, complex lithic and hafting technologies, personal ornamentation, pigment use and ‘symbolic’ art and artefacts; Henshilwood and Marean 2003 ; Mellars 2007 ) is far from unproblematic as a concept (Ames et al 2013 ), and many elements of such behaviour were also exhibited by archaic humans ( e.g. Zilhão et al 2010 ; Joordens et al 2015 ; Hoffmann et al 2018 ; Kissel and Fuentes 2018 ). Furthermore, prosocial motivations and behaviours, including care for the ill and injured (Spikins et al 2019 ), and collaborative hunting practices and food sharing (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al 2014 ; Agam and Barkai 2016 ; Faurby et al 2020 ), emerged relatively early in human evolution.…”
Section: Introduction—approaches To the ‘Modern Human Transition’mentioning
confidence: 99%