2014
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1408991111
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Behavioral and neural correlates of increased self-control in the absence of increased willpower

Abstract: People often exert willpower to choose a more valuable delayed reward over a less valuable immediate reward, but using willpower is taxing and frequently fails. In this research, we demonstrate the ability to enhance self-control (i.e., forgoing smaller immediate rewards in favor of larger delayed rewards) without exerting additional willpower. Using behavioral and neuroimaging data, we show that a reframing of rewards (i) reduced the subjective value of smaller immediate rewards relative to larger delayed rew… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

6
44
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(51 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
6
44
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Magen et al (2008) found that patience was increased by the Explicit zero frame-a phenomenon they called the Hidden zero effect. Similar results were earlier reported by Loewenstein andPrelec (1991, 1993), and later replicated by Radu et al (2011), Wu and He (2012), Read and Scholten (2012), and Magen et al (2014).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Magen et al (2008) found that patience was increased by the Explicit zero frame-a phenomenon they called the Hidden zero effect. Similar results were earlier reported by Loewenstein andPrelec (1991, 1993), and later replicated by Radu et al (2011), Wu and He (2012), Read and Scholten (2012), and Magen et al (2014).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Similarly, almost all previous studies of the Hidden zero effect have used stylized choices of this sort (Magen et al 2008(Magen et al , 2014Radu et al 2011;Read and Scholten 2012;Wu and He 2012). The only exception of which we are aware is the single study by Loewenstein and Prelec (1993) in which participants chose between dining options.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Although no interventions to increase grit in a clinical sample have been published, the technique of Duckworth and colleagues 8 to teach young students to identify plans to overcome obstacles in goal pursuit has been shown to moderately increase school attendance and academic performance. Furthermore, interventions to improve other self-regulation traits, including delay discounting 41 , self-control capacity 42 and distress tolerance 43 , have previously been efficacious. Whether grit can similarly be increased in patients with substance use disorders and whether increasing grit could improve treatment outcomes are promising areas for future research.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although the existence of such effects points to the possibility of intervening on choices to promote patient behavior, the feasibility of intervention is limited by the absence of a unifying explanation of the underlying mechanisms. In recent decades, a variety of explanations have been proposed for these effects, including shifting one’s reference points (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Loewenstein, 1988) or temporal focus (Lebreton et al, 2013; Lin, Horner, Bisby, & Burgess, 2015) or changing how outcomes are valued (Magen, Kim, Dweck, Gross, & Mcclure, 2014). However, the basic cognitive processes that give rise to these changes have remained elusive (Lempert & Phelps, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%