2021
DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2021.1874278
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of Simon and flanker conflict interference in younger and older adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

3
6
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 124 publications
3
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our study was elaborated based on the assumptions that the Simon effect is larger for older participants (Kawai et al, 2012;Kubo-Kawai & Kawai, 2010;Van der Lubbe & Verleger, 2002) and also considering evidences indicating cognitive declines in older adults (Nissim et al, 2017;Scrivano & Kieffaber, 2021;Wang et al, 2011). Actually, the magnitude of the Simon effect for older volunteers found herein (42 ms) showed a similar pattern to those reported by others.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our study was elaborated based on the assumptions that the Simon effect is larger for older participants (Kawai et al, 2012;Kubo-Kawai & Kawai, 2010;Van der Lubbe & Verleger, 2002) and also considering evidences indicating cognitive declines in older adults (Nissim et al, 2017;Scrivano & Kieffaber, 2021;Wang et al, 2011). Actually, the magnitude of the Simon effect for older volunteers found herein (42 ms) showed a similar pattern to those reported by others.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Researchers have described a larger Simon effect and slower RT in older adult volunteers when compared to young adults (Kawai et al, 2012;Kubo-Kawai & Kawai, 2010;Scrivano & Kieffaber, 2021;Van der Lubbe & Verleger, 2002). Some authors have suggested that an age-related cognitive decline impairs the suppression of automatic response activation to the irrelevant spatial attribute, leading to an enlarged Simon effect (Van der Lubbe & Verleger, 2002;Kubo-Kawai & Kawai, 2010;Kawai et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our hypotheses combined existing behavioral and ERP research demonstrating inhibition deficits in aging ( Vallesi et al, 2010 ; Vallesi, 2011 ; Hsieh and Fang, 2012 ; Kawai et al, 2012 ; Scrivano and Kieffaber, 2022 ) and behavioral and fMRI research demonstrating synchrony effects and TOD modulation of age-related differences in inhibitory efficiency ( May and Hasher, 1998 ; Borella et al, 2010 ; Anderson et al, 2014 ). First, we expected to replicate prior aging work showing poorer response inhibition and interference control in OAs relative to YAs, as reflected by behavioral indices (i.e., larger reaction time and accuracy difference scores, calculated from the difference between a basic processing condition and an inhibition condition), and neural indices (i.e., larger modulation of ERP components indexing inhibition).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Recent ERP research by Kan et al (2021) demonstrated that response inhibition and interference control, assessed through a hybrid Go-NoGo Flanker task, share similar cognitive processes in the early stages (i.e., N2), but exhibit different temporal mechanisms in the later stages (i.e., P3) of inhibitory processing. More specifically, the Go-NoGo P3 reflects the actual process of response inhibition (i.e., active suppression of the motor response, Band and Van Boxtel, 1999 ; Kok et al, 2004 ) and the Flanker P3 reflects interference control and resolution needed to negotiate the conflict response demands of the target stimulus ( Van ’t Ent, 2002 ; Scrivano and Kieffaber, 2022 ). According to the Inhibitory Deficit Theory ( Hasher and Zacks, 1988 ; Hasher and Campbell, 2020 ; Amer et al, 2022 ), older adults (OAs) are less able to suppress or ignore irrelevant thoughts and actions relative to younger adults (YAs).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation