1995
DOI: 10.1007/bf03162082
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight

Abstract: Advanced and novice readers in 1st grade and older disabled readers were given nonword reading and spelling tasks. In addition, they practiced learning to read simplified phonetic spellings of 16 words for several trials (e.g., 'messenger' spelled MESNGR, 'stupid' spelled STUPD). Following Reitsma's (1983) procedure, three days later subjects read originally learned spellings mixed with altered spellings in which single letters were added or deleted or replaced by phonetically equivalent or non-equivalent lett… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

20
127
2
4

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 149 publications
(156 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
20
127
2
4
Order By: Relevance
“…However, a decline in spelling performance for commonly used words may not occur at all. As reading researchers have noted (e.g., Ehri & Saltmarsh, 1995;Reitsma, 1983), even beginning readers need as few as three or four practices to retain letter information about specific words in memory. As a result, the information about specific words (such as you're or too) is quickly learned and provided it is encoded properly into LTM, it is not likely to be forgotten.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, a decline in spelling performance for commonly used words may not occur at all. As reading researchers have noted (e.g., Ehri & Saltmarsh, 1995;Reitsma, 1983), even beginning readers need as few as three or four practices to retain letter information about specific words in memory. As a result, the information about specific words (such as you're or too) is quickly learned and provided it is encoded properly into LTM, it is not likely to be forgotten.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, studies show that adults and children often remember the initial letters of words better than the subsequent letters (e.g., Jensen, 1962;Kooi, Schutz, & Baker, 1965;Treiman, Berch, & Weatherston, 1993). There is also evidence that letters at the edges of words, especially the initial letter, play a special role in reading for both children and adults (e.g., Ehri & Saltmarsh, 1995;Rayner, White, Johnson, & Liversedge, 2006;White, Johnson, Liversedge, & Rayner, 2008). For example, Rayner et al (2006) showed that, compared to a control condition in which words were spelled correctly, reading speed decreased more when a letter at the beginning of a word was switched with its neighbor (e.g., oculd for could) than when a letter in the middle or at the end of a word was switched (e.g., cuold or coudl for could).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The intervention was referred to as "graphosyllabic analysis" and resulted in improvements in the decoding of novel words, in the ability to segment words into syllables, and in the ability to identify subtle misspellings of common words where the erroneous letters were embedded in the middle of the words. This last outcome is important in that students with reading disabilities have been shown to attend primarily to the beginning and endings of words, causing them to make mistakes when sounding out polysyllabic words (Ehri & Saltmarsh, 1995).…”
Section: Intervention Strategies Phonics and Phonemic Awarenessmentioning
confidence: 99%