2017
DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00616
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Bayesian Inference of Two-Dimensional Contrast Sensitivity Function from Data Obtained with Classical One-Dimensional Algorithms Is Efficient

Abstract: The contrast sensitivity function that spans the two dimensions of contrast and spatial frequency is crucial in predicting functional vision both in research and clinical applications. In this study, the use of Bayesian inference was proposed to determine the parameters of the two-dimensional contrast sensitivity function. Two-dimensional Bayesian inference was extensively simulated in comparison to classical one-dimensional measures. Its performance on two-dimensional data gathered with different sampling alg… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
2
2

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In particular, we modeled the monotonic increase in discrimination performance with increasing stimulus contrast using Naka-Rushton functions (e.g., Herrmann et al, 2010;Huang & Dobkins, 2005;Ling & Carrasco, 2006b;Pestilli et al, 2009). Furthermore, for each eccentricity, we constrained the variation in contrast thresholds across SF by a functional form of the CSF that adheres to its conventional bandpass shape (Movshon & Kiorpes, 1988;Wang et al, 2017). In doing so, we greatly improved the parsimony of our modeling approach while conforming to known variations of contrast sensitivity across SF (Campbell & Robson, 1968;De Valois et al, 1974;Hilz & Cavonius, 1974;Kelly, 1977;Owsley, 2003;Robson, 1966;Robson & Graham, 1981;Rovamo et al, 1978;Virsu & Rovamo, 1979).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…In particular, we modeled the monotonic increase in discrimination performance with increasing stimulus contrast using Naka-Rushton functions (e.g., Herrmann et al, 2010;Huang & Dobkins, 2005;Ling & Carrasco, 2006b;Pestilli et al, 2009). Furthermore, for each eccentricity, we constrained the variation in contrast thresholds across SF by a functional form of the CSF that adheres to its conventional bandpass shape (Movshon & Kiorpes, 1988;Wang et al, 2017). In doing so, we greatly improved the parsimony of our modeling approach while conforming to known variations of contrast sensitivity across SF (Campbell & Robson, 1968;De Valois et al, 1974;Hilz & Cavonius, 1974;Kelly, 1977;Owsley, 2003;Robson, 1966;Robson & Graham, 1981;Rovamo et al, 1978;Virsu & Rovamo, 1979).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Neutral CSF . Contrast thresholds for all SFs in the Neutral condition were governed by a double-exponential function (adapted from Movshon & Kiorpes, 1988 ; Wang et al, 2017 ) of the form: where f csf defines the peak SF of the CSF (i.e., the SF where contrast sensitivity is highest) and s controls the slope of the function about its peak. The peak amplitude of the CSF ( γ csf ) is defined by the following: …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Methods have been proposed for accelerating CSF testing while preserving sufficient accuracy for clinical decision making (Gu et al, 2016; Lesmes et al, 2010; Wang et al, 2016). Because of intrinsic noisiness, speeding up the estimates typically requires making assumptions—incompletely justified in most cases—about some model parameters in order to reduce the degrees of freedom of the model to be learned (Treutwein & Strasburger, 1999).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%