2017
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-017-1230-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Bayes factor design analysis: Planning for compelling evidence

Abstract: A sizeable literature exists on the use of frequentist power analysis in the null-hypothesis significance testing (NHST) paradigm to facilitate the design of informative experiments. In contrast, there is almost no literature that discusses the design of experiments when Bayes factors (BFs) are used as a measure of evidence. Here we explore Bayes Factor Design Analysis (BFDA) as a useful tool to design studies for maximum efficiency and informativeness. We elaborate on three possible BF designs, (a) a fixed-n … Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
421
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 507 publications
(468 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
2
421
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For data analysis, we relied on Bayesian parameter estimation and Bayesian hypothesis tests by Bayes Factors (BF; (Körber, Radlmayr, & Bengler, 2016;Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, & Iverson, 2009;Schönbrodt & Wagenmakers, 2017). A BF represents the ratio of the probability of the data given a null model to the probability of the data given an alternative model and thereby offers a gradual quantification of evidence (Schönbrodt, Wagenmakers, Zehetleitner, & Perugini, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For data analysis, we relied on Bayesian parameter estimation and Bayesian hypothesis tests by Bayes Factors (BF; (Körber, Radlmayr, & Bengler, 2016;Rouder, Speckman, Sun, Morey, & Iverson, 2009;Schönbrodt & Wagenmakers, 2017). A BF represents the ratio of the probability of the data given a null model to the probability of the data given an alternative model and thereby offers a gradual quantification of evidence (Schönbrodt, Wagenmakers, Zehetleitner, & Perugini, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ethical approval for the study was granted at all three universities. Based on our design analysis (see below), we applied a sequential data collection protocol (Schönbrodt & Wagenmakers, 2018;Schönbrodt, Wagenmakers, Zehetleitner, & Perugini, 2017) and set out to collect between at least 120 and maximum 192 participants (a minimum of 20 and maximum of 32 participants per stimulation condition and study site). Subjects who failed to provide a complete dataset for technical (e.g., failure of the equipment) or other reasons (e.g., experiment not completed) were excluded from the analysis and replaced by new subjects.…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, we did not want to collect more data than necessary for ethical reasons. Therefore, we chose to apply a sequential design with a specified maximum sample size of N = 192 (Schönbrodt & Wagenmakers, 2018;Schönbrodt et al, 2017). In order to avoid spurious rejections of the existence of an effect, we chose to first collect a minimum sample size of N = 120 (20 per lab and condition).…”
Section: Design Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A weakly informed Cauchy prior with rscale = √ 2/2 was used for H 1 (set by default by the "ttestBF" function). Referring to Schönbrodt and Wagenmakers (2018), we interpreted a BF > 3 as at least "moderate" evidence of H 1 (i.e., the hypothesis that the two means are not equal at the population level), and a BF < 1/3 as "moderate" evidence of H 0 (i.e., the hypothesis that the two means are equal at the population level). Any BF coming between these two cutoffs was regarded as "indecisive" evidence.…”
Section: Descriptive Statisticsmentioning
confidence: 99%