1983
DOI: 10.1016/s0003-3472(83)80026-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Battery hens name their price: Consumer demand theory and the measurement of ethological ‘needs’

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
144
0
12

Year Published

2003
2003
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 368 publications
(162 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
6
144
0
12
Order By: Relevance
“…The weight at which each mouse failed to enter the ENR cage for 48 h was deemed its 'maximum price paid'. This maximum price was used to infer each mouse's level of motivation for enrichment [7,28,46]: essentially a 'break-point' measure, but assessed over a longer time period than as typically utilized in conventional operant set-ups (cf. e.g.…”
Section: Transfer To Non-enriched Standard Conditions and Subsequentmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The weight at which each mouse failed to enter the ENR cage for 48 h was deemed its 'maximum price paid'. This maximum price was used to infer each mouse's level of motivation for enrichment [7,28,46]: essentially a 'break-point' measure, but assessed over a longer time period than as typically utilized in conventional operant set-ups (cf. e.g.…”
Section: Transfer To Non-enriched Standard Conditions and Subsequentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We then increased the weight daily and recorded the number of visits to the food cage each day until the mice did not enter the food cage for 24 h. The aim of this final phase was to use food as a benchmark resource (e.g. [7]), to estimate the maximum weight each mouse was able to push. Controlling for this should ideally help reduce variation in the maximum price for enrichments that merely reflected individual differences in physical strength.…”
Section: Transfer To Non-enriched Standard Conditions and Subsequentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dawkins, 1980Dawkins, , 1983Poole, 1992;Veasey et al, 1996Price, 1999 who concluded that it would be inappropriate to assume captive animals would require the same elements in their environment as their wild conspecifics.…”
Section: Behavioural Needs: Definitions Concepts and Some Characterimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Methods to assess the animal's appraisal of its situation are: (i) assessing the price the animal is willing to pay for access as addressed by consumer-demand studies (e.g. Dawkins, 1983;Mason et al, , 1999Mason, 1997, 2000) and ( Theoretically, anticipatory behaviour preceding an incentive can be induced without previous experience of swimming water. Thus, this method can be applied to assess and compare the sensitivity of the reward system of mink in the presence, absence and after deprivation of a swimming bath, and so, may give some insight into the effects of experience on swimming motivation ("can you miss what you do not know?").…”
Section: Stereotypies In the Presence And Absence Of A Water Bathmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Preference tests have thus often been used to reveal what animals choose for improved well-being. For example, chickens prefer larger cages with a substrate to smaller cages with wire floors [Dawkins, 1983], while naturally semi-aquatic American mink will push door-weights as heavy as those they will push to gain food, to reach water in which they can swim and ''head-dip'' [Mason et al, 2001]. Negative stimuli can be evaluated too; for example, the avoidance of potentially noxious gasses can be assessed [Cooper et al, 1998;Jones et al, 2003], while the aversiveness of pain to lame poultry has been revealed by their selection of food dosed with analgesics over unadulterated food [e.g., Danbury et al, 2000].…”
Section: Preference and Avoidancementioning
confidence: 99%