2012
DOI: 10.1037/a0029779
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Backloading in the sequential lineup prevents within-lineup criterion shifts that undermine eyewitness identification performance.

Abstract: Although the sequential lineup has been proposed as a means of protecting innocent suspects from mistaken identification, little is known about the importance of various aspects of the procedure. One potentially important detail is that witnesses should not know how many people are in the lineup. This is sometimes achieved by backloading the lineup so that witnesses believe that the lineup includes more photographs than it actually does. This study aimed to investigate the effect of backloading on witness deci… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
66
2
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(82 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
12
66
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Typically, when the target (perpetrator) is present, the correct identification rate (or hit rate) is higher in the simultaneous condition, whereas when the target is absent, the false identification rate (or false alarm rate) is lower in the sequential condition (Clark, Howell, & Davey, 2008;Steblay et al, 2001;Steblay, Dysart, & Wells, 2011). These results have been found in event memory studies (e.g., Lindsay & Wells, 1985), in standard face recognition paradigms (e.g., Meissner et al, 2005) and in incidental learning tasks (e.g., Palmer & Brewer, 2012). It has been argued that performance was "superior" in a sequential ineup because the diagnosticity ratio, or the ratio of target to innocent suspect identifications, tends to be larger in the sequential condition (e.g., Steblay et al, 2011).…”
supporting
confidence: 64%
“…Typically, when the target (perpetrator) is present, the correct identification rate (or hit rate) is higher in the simultaneous condition, whereas when the target is absent, the false identification rate (or false alarm rate) is lower in the sequential condition (Clark, Howell, & Davey, 2008;Steblay et al, 2001;Steblay, Dysart, & Wells, 2011). These results have been found in event memory studies (e.g., Lindsay & Wells, 1985), in standard face recognition paradigms (e.g., Meissner et al, 2005) and in incidental learning tasks (e.g., Palmer & Brewer, 2012). It has been argued that performance was "superior" in a sequential ineup because the diagnosticity ratio, or the ratio of target to innocent suspect identifications, tends to be larger in the sequential condition (e.g., Steblay et al, 2011).…”
supporting
confidence: 64%
“…We did not draw participants' attention to the number of lineup members in our sequential lineups and participants completed a randomized assortment of target-present and target-absent trials. An effect of lineup size may emerge if lineup size is more salient (e.g., Horry et al, 2012;Lindsay et al, 1991), but this could be countered by varying lineup size across trials.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Lindsay, Lea, and Fulford (1991) and Horry et al (2012) found correct rejections (but not correct identifications) were lower when participants were aware of how many members comprised their sequential lineup. A participant who rejected a sequential lineup in a multiple-trial experiment would discover how many lineup members comprised the lineups, which could lead them to adopt a more lenient criterion for identification as they approached the end of subsequent lineups.…”
Section: Possible Interactions With Multiple Trialsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To see why, I turn next to the issue of the reliability of eyewitness identification and to the lab-based recognition memory tasks that are most commonly used to investigate it. For decades, this research has been mostly carried out without regard for the distinction between discriminability and response bias (for some notable exceptions, see Ebbesen & Flowe, 2002;Horry, Palmer, & Brewer, 2012;Meissner, Tredoux, Parker, & MacLin, 2005;, and the reported results have had a profound effect on practices in the legal system. Without the guidance of basic theories of recognition memory (theories that protect one from compelling but often faulty intuitions), the argument can be made that eyewitness identification researchers got it wrong in several ways .…”
Section: To the Courtroommentioning
confidence: 99%