1989
DOI: 10.3758/bf03334586
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Awareness of attention allocation and time estimation accuracy

Abstract: Subjects performed a psychomotoric task and gave a prospective verbal estimate of its duration. One group of subjects was then made aware of optimal division of attention between the psychomotoric task and the passage of time, and again performed the task and prospectively estimated its duration. It was found that the accuracy of the time estimation markedly improved for that group of subjects relative to a control group whose awareness of the division of attention was not raised. Implications for understandin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
15
0
2

Year Published

1992
1992
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
1
15
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Conversely, the ratio generally decreases as a function of the amount of information processed in a distracting secondary task (e.g., Grondin & Macar, 1992;Zakay & Tsal, 1989). We agree with the traditional view (e.g., Creelman, 1962;Thomas & Weaver, 1975;Treisman, 1963) of time perception, according to which perceived duration is a function of the amount of information processed per unit of objective time.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 77%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Conversely, the ratio generally decreases as a function of the amount of information processed in a distracting secondary task (e.g., Grondin & Macar, 1992;Zakay & Tsal, 1989). We agree with the traditional view (e.g., Creelman, 1962;Thomas & Weaver, 1975;Treisman, 1963) of time perception, according to which perceived duration is a function of the amount of information processed per unit of objective time.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 77%
“…Note that in Experiment 2, where the standards were moving and the oddball was stationary, the temporal expansion factor was only about 1.2, which is similar to the degree of expansion we find here. Lastly, the counting that the observers carried out for oddball stimuli probably worked against TSE, because a nontemporal concurrent processing task is known to decrease perceived duration, rather than increase it (e.g., Grondin & Macar, 1992;Hicks & Brundige, 1974;Hicks et al, 1976;Hülser, 1924;Katz, 1906;Macar et al, 1994;Predebon, 1996;Quasebarth, 1924;Thomas & Cantor, 1978;Underwood & Swain, 1973;Zakay, 1993;Zakay & Tsal, 1989).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, in the classical studies ofKatz (1906), intervals ofabout 1.2 sec werejudged shorterwhen attention was distracted by an accompanying task (e.g., reading ofsyllables). Meanwhile, numerous studies have replicated and extended this basic finding with various dual-task paradigms (Grondin & Macar, 1992;Hülser, 1924; Macar, Grondin, & Casini, 1994;Predebon, 1996;Quasebarth, 1924;Thomas & Cantor, 1978;Underwood & Swain, 1973;Zakay, 1993;Zakay & Tsal, 1989).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The opposite is true for retrospective judgments. Also, when people are cognitively engaged and isolated from temporal stimuli, time is perceived to move more quickly (Vroon, 1970;Zakay, Nitzan, & Glicksohn, 1983;Zakay & Tsal, 1989). We have all experienced this basic empirical finding: time passes faster when there are more distractions.…”
Section: Time Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 90%