2004
DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.06.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Avian olfactory navigation: its empirical foundation and conceptual state

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
129
1
1

Year Published

2005
2005
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 160 publications
(134 citation statements)
references
References 78 publications
1
129
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our findings suggest that, similar to honeybees and pigeons (3,15,16), bats are able to home to one of two goal locations-trees or cavesindicating flexible navigational abilities. However, these results cannot help distinguish between map-based navigation guided by large-scale odor gradients (24) or magnetic gradients (25), versus navigation using cognitive-map mechanisms relying on selftriangulation based on distal visual landmarks (26).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our findings suggest that, similar to honeybees and pigeons (3,15,16), bats are able to home to one of two goal locations-trees or cavesindicating flexible navigational abilities. However, these results cannot help distinguish between map-based navigation guided by large-scale odor gradients (24) or magnetic gradients (25), versus navigation using cognitive-map mechanisms relying on selftriangulation based on distal visual landmarks (26).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The most detailed investigations of orientation by olfactory cues in birds have been done on pigeons, swifts and starlings (Wallraff 2004). While considerable evidence suggests that pigeons, in particular, can use olfactory maps to assist them in navigating over large (hundreds of square kilometres) spatial scales, olfactory orientation has often been challenged on purely conceptual grounds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Obviously, the question of turbulence looms large, yet animals are highly adapted to decode turbulence (18)(19)(20), and odorant distributions may be stable, even in air (21). Olfactory systems are also notably integrated with mechanosensory systems to measure turbulence, such as vibrissae (mammals), antennae (insects), antennules (crustaceans), and lateral lines (fish) (22,23).…”
Section: Convergence In Olfactory System Structure and Functionmentioning
confidence: 99%