2015
DOI: 10.1111/jav.00593
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Avian compass systems: do all migratory species possess all three?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Some of his birds which had learned an incorrect celestial rotational axis did not refer to the proper rotational axis in the following year despite having had access to natural celestial rotation during the whole summer, but kept orienting by their learnt “north” star 5 . Emlen’s birds were indigo buntings and ours were European robins and even though the basic orientation mechanisms are probably identical in most night-migratory songbird species, the exact hierarchy and/or calibration rules may be species-specific 16 17 20 41 . Thus, species differences could potentially explain the differences between Emlen’s and our results.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Some of his birds which had learned an incorrect celestial rotational axis did not refer to the proper rotational axis in the following year despite having had access to natural celestial rotation during the whole summer, but kept orienting by their learnt “north” star 5 . Emlen’s birds were indigo buntings and ours were European robins and even though the basic orientation mechanisms are probably identical in most night-migratory songbird species, the exact hierarchy and/or calibration rules may be species-specific 16 17 20 41 . Thus, species differences could potentially explain the differences between Emlen’s and our results.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…This diversity of responses seems to parallel the situation with compass systems, where not all species (and populations?) seem to use all potentially available compass cues equally well [16]. It cries for more research into navigational maps of different migrant species, ideally in different parts of the world.…”
Section: Plos Onementioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, firstly, as already mentioned, there is no direct evidence that these songbird migrants use the stellar compass but not the magnetic one during nocturnal flights (Chernetsov, 2015). Secondly, this theory cannot explain the results of the study by Chernetsov et al (2011), in which birds had access to the BMP and landmarks at sunset during the cue conflict and then were released after the stars appeared in the sky but birds did not show any type of calibration. The suggestion by Sjöberg and Muheim (2016) that it could be the topographic bias in the departure direction of song thrushes released on the Courish Spit is not tenable, because the data from another release site in mainland Kaliningrad region showed the same departure direction after release (Chernetsov et al, 2011).…”
Section: Animal Behaviourmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…2. Three types of responses to cue-conflict treatments in migratory birds, when exposed to changed magnetic field (-120° horizontally turned) under natural clear sky during autumn migration: A) Magnetic cues calibrate celestial compasses; B) Celestial cues calibrate the magnetic compass; C) The stellar or the magnetic cues are used separately without transferring information to other compasses (simple dominance of one cue, from Chernetsov et al, 2011). The black arrows represent the horizontal direction of (geo)magnetic field (the direction to magnetic North), the red arrow represents the expected direction of the birds, the blue arrow represents the expected direction of the birds if they use the magnetic compass as a primary cue reference (C, cue-conflict phase).…”
Section: ) Magnetic Cues Calibrate Celestial Compassesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation