1976
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1976.26-451
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

AUTOSHAPING, RANDOM CONTROL, AND OMISSION TRAINING IN THE RAT1

Abstract: The role of the stimulus-reinforcer contingency in the development and maintenance of lever contact responding was studied in hooded rats. In Experiment I, three groups of experimentally naive rats were trained either on autoshaping, omission training, or a random-control procedure. Subjects trained by the autoshaping procedure responded more consistently than did either random-control or omission-trained subjects. The probability of at least one lever contact per trial was slightly higher in subjects trained … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

6
48
1

Year Published

1979
1979
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(55 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
6
48
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus 60% of the acquired sign-tracking CR persisted despite the omission schedule, and the persistence was similar to Controls. This suggests that the CS+ retained much of its motivational value and that its expression in behavior was highly flexible.Although Locurto et al (1976) observed differences in response topography between rats placed on sign-tracking and omission schedules, these differences were not quantified. Based on a near-full reduction in lever contacts in their study, Locurto et al (1976) argued that omission training was ineffective at maintaining responding to the lever.…”
mentioning
confidence: 89%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Thus 60% of the acquired sign-tracking CR persisted despite the omission schedule, and the persistence was similar to Controls. This suggests that the CS+ retained much of its motivational value and that its expression in behavior was highly flexible.Although Locurto et al (1976) observed differences in response topography between rats placed on sign-tracking and omission schedules, these differences were not quantified. Based on a near-full reduction in lever contacts in their study, Locurto et al (1976) argued that omission training was ineffective at maintaining responding to the lever.…”
mentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Sign-tracking is a key model for studying behavioral and neural mechanisms of normal and excessive motivational attraction to reward-paired stimuli (Lajoie and Bindra 1976;Berridge 2004;Tomie et al 2008;Flagel et al 2010;Robinson and Berridge 2013;Huys et al 2014;Robinson et al 2014). Negative automaintenance, in which a lever press cancels reward, has been used to show that sign-tracking can be markedly sensitive to instrumental contingency changes (Williams and Williams 1969;Stiers and Silberberg 1974;Locurto et al 1976), suggesting a sensitivity to response-reward associations (Skinner 1992). For example, Locurto et al (1976) have found that lever contacts after sign-tracking are markedly reduced in rats moved to an omission schedule, with similar rates of lever contacts compared with rats exposed to extinction or random cue/reward delivery.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Peterson maintained his rats at 80070 of their normal body weights by giving them supplemental feedings of 5 to 10 g daily. They had free access to water in the home cage, but no water in the test chamber-the standard practice of subsequent experiments on autoshaping with food delivery as the signaled event (Atnip, 1977;Locurto, Terrace, & Gibbon, 1976;Stiers & Silberberg, 1974).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%