2008
DOI: 10.3758/pbr.15.1.108
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Automaticity in motor sequence learning does not impair response inhibition

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
39
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
6
39
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The statistical analysis of electrical source estimations showed that this effect followed from a decrease in the activity of the rIFG as well as of the pre-SMA, primary motor cortex and basal ganglia. Our behavioral results corroborate previous psychophysical evidence for an improvement of SST performance with practice (Fillmore et al, 2001;Turner et al, 2004; though see Cohen and Poldrack, 2008). Because it takes into account both the SSD and Go RT, the SSRT is generally considered as being independent of changes in response strategies and thus to constitute a reliable index of SST proficiency (Congdon et al, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…The statistical analysis of electrical source estimations showed that this effect followed from a decrease in the activity of the rIFG as well as of the pre-SMA, primary motor cortex and basal ganglia. Our behavioral results corroborate previous psychophysical evidence for an improvement of SST performance with practice (Fillmore et al, 2001;Turner et al, 2004; though see Cohen and Poldrack, 2008). Because it takes into account both the SSD and Go RT, the SSRT is generally considered as being independent of changes in response strategies and thus to constitute a reliable index of SST proficiency (Congdon et al, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…For example, training IC with stop-signal tasks (SST) or Go/ NoGo tasks has been found to decrease stop-signal reaction times (SSRT; Guerrieri et al 2012;Manuel et al 2013), or to decrease false alarm rate and/or response time in the trained task, respectively (Schapkin et al 2007;Manuel et al 2010;Johnstone et al 2012;Benikos et al 2013). A few studies, however, did not find such a decrease (e.g., Cohen and Poldrack, 2008), or only in some conditions (Ditye et al 2012). These patterns of behavioral improvements have been hypothesized to follow from improvements in the speed of inhibition processes (White et al 2014;Chavan et al 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…On the other hand, it may be that the signal was entirely ineffective in associating with the stop outcome, and that the only associations in play were those involving the cue. Supporting this view, research assessing how the relative speed of the stop process (as indexed by the SSRT) changes with practice has been mixed and has not always yielded any significant improvement (Cohen & Poldrack, 2008;Logan & Burkell, 1986). This suggests that response inhibition may not benefit from acquired associations between stop signals and stopping.…”
Section: Associative Learning Of Stop Signalsmentioning
confidence: 99%