1996
DOI: 10.1080/026999396380330
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Automatic Affective Appraisal of Words

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
36
1

Year Published

1998
1998
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
5
36
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Instead, affect appears to be processed by top-down networks. Similar results have been found when emotional faces were used in a modified Posner cueing paradigm (Fox, Russo, Bowles, & Dutton, 2001) and when threat-related words and faces were used in a variation of the Stroop task (White, 1996). Moreover, examining the affective pronunciation priming task, De Houwer and Randell (2002) observed affective priming only when attention was focused on the primes.…”
Section: The Affective Automaticity Hypothesissupporting
confidence: 62%
“…Instead, affect appears to be processed by top-down networks. Similar results have been found when emotional faces were used in a modified Posner cueing paradigm (Fox, Russo, Bowles, & Dutton, 2001) and when threat-related words and faces were used in a variation of the Stroop task (White, 1996). Moreover, examining the affective pronunciation priming task, De Houwer and Randell (2002) observed affective priming only when attention was focused on the primes.…”
Section: The Affective Automaticity Hypothesissupporting
confidence: 62%
“…The results of our first four experiments suggest that processes involved in the disengagement from threat distinguish high and low state-anxious people. Threatening stimuli that appear in unattended locations do not attract visual attention any more than neutral stimuli (Fox, 1994;White, 1996), but once detected, they are processed more deeply and take longer to disengage from than neutral stimuli.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In light of the evidence reviewed previously showing that the presence of threat-related stimuli in unattended locations does not involuntarily draw attention (Fox, 1994;White, 1996), it is predicted that attentional bias toward threat may be due to defective disengagement from threatening stimuli. Thus, we predict a difference on the disengage component of attention (i.e., invalidly cued trials), which should be stronger for high state-anxious participants than for low state-anxious participants.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Anderson & Phelps, 2001;Zajonc, 1980). With regard to lexical stimuli, this possibility may be considered unlikely because language has developed only recently in human evolution, hence it could be argued that both hemispheres may be unable to extract the semantics of language without prior lexical analysis (White, 1996). On the other hand, some researchers have in fact reported brain asymmetries in the processing of emotional words that were presented below the subjective identification threshold (Làvadas, Cimatti, Del Pesce, & Tuozzi, 1993;Wexler, Warrenburg, Schwartz, & Janer, 1992).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%