2004
DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.045
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Auditory-visual temporal integration as a function of distance: no compensation for sound-transmission time in human perception

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
62
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(74 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
9
62
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Importantly, these early effects of audio-visual integration are highly sensitive to asynchronies between the sensory signals (Sekuler et al, 1997;van der Burg, Olivers, et al, 2008). In order to compensate for offsets in stimulus processing as well as physical transfer times (Lewald & Guski, 2004), audio-visual integration during perceptual tasks tolerates small deviations from perfect synchrony (e.g., Vroomen & Keetels, 2010). Although this temporal window for audiovisual integration varies across different types of stimuli (e.g., Fujisaki, Shimojo, Kashino, & Nishida, 2004;van Wassenhove, et al, 2007), it typically does not exceed a few hundred milliseconds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, these early effects of audio-visual integration are highly sensitive to asynchronies between the sensory signals (Sekuler et al, 1997;van der Burg, Olivers, et al, 2008). In order to compensate for offsets in stimulus processing as well as physical transfer times (Lewald & Guski, 2004), audio-visual integration during perceptual tasks tolerates small deviations from perfect synchrony (e.g., Vroomen & Keetels, 2010). Although this temporal window for audiovisual integration varies across different types of stimuli (e.g., Fujisaki, Shimojo, Kashino, & Nishida, 2004;van Wassenhove, et al, 2007), it typically does not exceed a few hundred milliseconds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For audiovisual speech, sensitivity of temporal order was again worse for the gender-congruent than for the gender-incongruent trials, but there was no congruency effect for the monkey calls or for the human voice imitating the coos. also found no congruency effect for audiovisual music and object events that either matched (e.g., the sight of a note being played on a piano together with the corresponding sound, the video of a bouncing ball with a 2005; Heron, Whitaker, McGraw, & Horoshenkov, 2007;Lewald & Guski, 2004;Stone et al, 2001). See Table 1 for an overview.…”
Section: Compensation For External Factorsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Others, however, failed to observe compensation for distance (Arnold et al, 2005;Heron et al, 2007;Lewald & Guski, 2004;Stone et al, 2001). For example, Lewald and Guski (2004) used a rather wide range of distances (1,5,10,20, and 50 m), but their audio-visual stimuli (a sequence of five beeps/flashes) were delivered by colocated speakers/LEDs placed in the open field.…”
Section: Temporal Recalibrationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The similarity of TOT across sensory modalities for simple stimuli is surprising given the differences of neural latencies between audition (Celesia 1976;Lakatos et al 2005) and vision (Buchner et al 1997;Schroeder 1998;Zeki 2001). Several groups have suggested the possibility of mechanisms compensating for the neural delays across the senses (Engel & Dougherty 1971;Sugita & Suzuki 2003;Kopinska & Harris 2004), but they remain controversial (Lewald & Guski 2004;Arnold et al 2005) and may not take place below the 'horizon of simultaneity', i.e. when auditory and visual sources are less than 10 m away from the observer (Pö ppel 1988;Pö ppel et al 1990).…”
Section: Two-way Non-identity Problemmentioning
confidence: 99%