2009
DOI: 10.1121/1.4784171
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Auditory-phonetic projection and lexical structure in the recognition of sine-wave words.

Abstract: Speech remains intelligible despite the elimination of canonical acoustic correlates of phonemes from the spectrum. Listeners tolerate distortion or spectral blur in tone analogs, noise band vocoded speech, and acoustic chimeras in utterances ranging from syllables to isolated words and sentences. A portion of this flexibility is attributable to short-term perceptual learning in auditory-to-phonetic projection, though exploiting the properties of lexical neighborhoods plays a role with utterances composed of w… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The behavioral results of the present study are consistent with previous findings showing that speech recognition is influenced by the acoustic characteristics of speakers. A number of behavioral findings suggested that attributes of a speaker's voice are perceived and memorized along with the speech message (Bradlow et al, 1999;Palmeri et al, 1993;Pisoni, 1993) and that knowledge about the vocal characteristics of a speaker enhances comprehension of what is said (Best et al, 2008;Bradlow and Pisoni, 1999;Kitterick et al, 2010;Levi et al, 2011;Nygaard and Pisoni, 1998;Nygaard et al, 1994;Remez et al, 2009;Yonan and Sommers, 2000). Conversely, understanding speech in the context of speaker changes typically results in performance costs and longer processing times (e.g., Magnuson and Nusbaum, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The behavioral results of the present study are consistent with previous findings showing that speech recognition is influenced by the acoustic characteristics of speakers. A number of behavioral findings suggested that attributes of a speaker's voice are perceived and memorized along with the speech message (Bradlow et al, 1999;Palmeri et al, 1993;Pisoni, 1993) and that knowledge about the vocal characteristics of a speaker enhances comprehension of what is said (Best et al, 2008;Bradlow and Pisoni, 1999;Kitterick et al, 2010;Levi et al, 2011;Nygaard and Pisoni, 1998;Nygaard et al, 1994;Remez et al, 2009;Yonan and Sommers, 2000). Conversely, understanding speech in the context of speaker changes typically results in performance costs and longer processing times (e.g., Magnuson and Nusbaum, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Error bars represent standard errors of the difference scores. Remez et al, 2011). Note, however, that under everyday listening conditions, infants typically receive a large proportion of their language input from speakers who spend a lot of time together and thus may exhibit this type of vocal convergence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This was done using Praat software (version 6.12, from http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/download_win.html) and a script written by Chris Darwin (http://www.lifesci.sussex.ac.uk/home/Chris_Darwin/Praatscripts/SWS). While it is well established that the intelligibility of algorithmically-constructed sine-wave speech is inferior to that constructed by hand 40 , for our purposes this script was sufficient to create a stimulus that is intelligible to informed listeners, but not to naïve listeners. The resulting sine-wave speech sentences (Fig.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…4A) for this purpose, a stimulus created from sets of three sine waves that track the amplitude and frequency of speech formants 39 . Sine-wave speech is of particular interest for this purpose as its intelligibility can be very quickly and dramatically affected by perceptual learning: Naïve listeners, who have never been exposed to sine-wave speech, typically perceive the stimulus as non-speech sounds, such as whistles or bird songs, whereas listeners trained to perceive it as speech can often identify the different words in the stimulus 40 . This property of sine-wave speech has been used previously in, for example, brain imaging experiments to compare brain responses to identical stimuli that are heard as speech or non-speech [42][43][44] .…”
Section: Experiments 3: Effect Of Intelligibilitymentioning
confidence: 99%