1987
DOI: 10.1080/00029157.1987.10402720
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attributions, Involuntariness, and Hypnotic Rapport

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
10
0

Year Published

1987
1987
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
2
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In keeping with the hypothesis that involuntariness reports are associated with occurrence schemata, research has shown that subjects' ratings of involuntariness are associated with the belief that their hypnotic behavior is a function of the hypnotist's ability and effort (Lynn, Snodgrass, Hardaway, & Lenz, 1984;Lynn, Snodgrass, Rhue, Nash, & Frauman, 1987). In one study (Lynn, Snodgrass, Rhue, Nash, & Frauman, 1987), subjects' posthypnotic attributions of response causality to the hypnotist's ability and effort were found to be associated with posthypnotic ratings of involuntariness. In a second study (Lynn, Snodgrass, Hardaway, & Lenz, 1984), subjects' prehyp-This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.…”
Section: Situational Representationsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…In keeping with the hypothesis that involuntariness reports are associated with occurrence schemata, research has shown that subjects' ratings of involuntariness are associated with the belief that their hypnotic behavior is a function of the hypnotist's ability and effort (Lynn, Snodgrass, Hardaway, & Lenz, 1984;Lynn, Snodgrass, Rhue, Nash, & Frauman, 1987). In one study (Lynn, Snodgrass, Rhue, Nash, & Frauman, 1987), subjects' posthypnotic attributions of response causality to the hypnotist's ability and effort were found to be associated with posthypnotic ratings of involuntariness. In a second study (Lynn, Snodgrass, Hardaway, & Lenz, 1984), subjects' prehyp-This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.…”
Section: Situational Representationsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Nevertheless, certain characteristics of an experimenter or clinician might enable them to be more effective in administering suggestions, such as the ability to present oneself as a convincing authority (Woody & Szechtman, 2007), or the ability to develop rapport with a participant (Lynn, Snodgrass, Rhue, Nash, & Frauman, 1987). Still, these skills do not require extensive training.…”
Section: Myths and Misconceptions About Inducing Hypnosismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is occasionally anecdotally reported either that women or men are more effective hypnotists, although data indicate that hypnotic suggestibility is not related to a hypnotist's gender (Coe, 1976; Nash & Spinler, 1989). Nevertheless, certain characteristics of an experimenter or clinician might enable them to be more effective in administering suggestions, such as the ability to present oneself as a convincing authority (Woody & Szechtman, 2007), or the ability to develop rapport with a participant (Lynn, Snodgrass, Rhue, Nash, & Frauman, 1987). Still, these skills do not require extensive training.…”
Section: Myths and Misconceptions About Inducing Hypnosismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore several studies indicate that hypnotist/subject rapport influences hypnotic responding [18][19][20][21]. For instance, it was found in one study [6] that the degree of hypnotizability gain that followed CSTP administration was positively correlated with the degree of rapport reported by subjects toward their trainers.…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%