2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.060
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attention, predictive learning, and the inverse base-rate effect: Evidence from event-related potentials

Abstract: We report the first electrophysiological investigation of the inverse base-rate effect (IBRE), a robust non-rational bias in predictive learning. In the IBRE, participants learn that one pair of symptoms (AB) predicts a frequently occurring disease, whilst an overlapping pair of symptoms (AC) predicts a rarely occurring disease. Participants subsequently infer that BC predicts the rare disease, a non-rational decision made in opposition to the underlying base rates of the two diseases. Error-driven attention t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
74
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(85 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
7
74
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Results showed that attention in the search task was strongly biased toward stimuli that observers experienced as relevant for the categorization task, although participants had the opportunity to prepare for short-term suppression of such stimuli (Experiment 1 and 2) or to fully adjust their attentional control settings altogether (Experiment 3). The findings are well in line with other studies reporting a selection bias toward stimuli with high predictive value (Feldmann-W€ ustefeld et al, 2015;Le Pelley, Beesley, & Griffiths, 2011;Le Pelley, Calvini, & Spears, 2013;Lucke, Lachnit, Koenig, & Uengoer, 2013;Mitchell, Griffiths, Seetoo, & Lovibond, 2012;Wills, Lavric, Croft, & Hodgson, 2007;Wills, Lavric, Hemmings, & Surrey, 2014), and further emphasize the role of prior experience in guiding attention. The present study shows that selection history can bias initial attention deployment very persistently, even when this is detrimental to the task performance and cannot be overruled by top-down attentional control, even under ideal preparation conditions.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Results showed that attention in the search task was strongly biased toward stimuli that observers experienced as relevant for the categorization task, although participants had the opportunity to prepare for short-term suppression of such stimuli (Experiment 1 and 2) or to fully adjust their attentional control settings altogether (Experiment 3). The findings are well in line with other studies reporting a selection bias toward stimuli with high predictive value (Feldmann-W€ ustefeld et al, 2015;Le Pelley, Beesley, & Griffiths, 2011;Le Pelley, Calvini, & Spears, 2013;Lucke, Lachnit, Koenig, & Uengoer, 2013;Mitchell, Griffiths, Seetoo, & Lovibond, 2012;Wills, Lavric, Croft, & Hodgson, 2007;Wills, Lavric, Hemmings, & Surrey, 2014), and further emphasize the role of prior experience in guiding attention. The present study shows that selection history can bias initial attention deployment very persistently, even when this is detrimental to the task performance and cannot be overruled by top-down attentional control, even under ideal preparation conditions.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…They found that attentional preference for the perfect predictors of rare outcomes predicted stronger inverse base-rate effects. Another inverse base-rate study also demonstrated an attentional preference for rare associations as measured via ERP correlates of selective attention (Wills, Lavric, Hemmings, & Surrey, 2014). Another study that was very similar to the present ones, but showing the inverse baserate effect in the formation of group impressions (i.e., stereotypes), showed that participants paid more attention to rare rather than common behaviors when learning about minority group members (Sherman et al, 2009).…”
Section: Limitationssupporting
confidence: 85%
“…The selection of time windows for ERP analysis followed two steps. First we identified the components of interest from previous literature analyzing the ERPs elicited by predictive visual stimuli presented in a nonlateralized fashion: N1, SN, P2, SRP, and P3 (Baker & Holroyd, 2009;Dunning & Hajcak, 2007;Holroyd et al 2011;Liao, Gramann, Feng, Deak, & Li, 2011;Schevernels et al, 2014;Wills et al, 2007Wills et al, , 2014. Then, we identified these components from inspection of ERP waveforms and topographical maps.…”
Section: Event-related Potential Data Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, people spend longer looking at predictive stimuli than nonpredictive stimuli (Le Pelley, Beesley, & Griffiths, 2011;Wills, Lavric, Croft, & Hodgson, 2007), people are faster to respond to events occurring in the location of predictive stimuli (Le Pelley, Vadillo, & Luque, 2013), and people are faster to learn new information about predictive stimuli (Le Pelley & McLaren, 2003). Notably, using EEG, Wills et al (2007; see also Wills, Lavric, Hemmings, & Surrey, 2014) found that the magnitude of the anterior N1 and selection negativity (SN) ERP component-which has previously been characterized as an index of visual attentional processes (Hillyard & Anllo-Vento, 1998)-differed between predictive and nonpredictive stimuli. However, in all of these prior studies, the relationship between S and R was confounded with the relationship between S and O.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%