2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.humov.2020.102716
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attention focus modulates afferent input to motor cortex during skilled action

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Behavioral effects have dominated the attentional focus literature. Some, although more limited, attention has been paid to potential neural or brain-related underpinnings of the EF advantage, particularly those in motor cortex and somatosensory areas (Kuhn et al, 2018; Kuhn et al, 2017; Suzuki & Meehan, 2020; Zentgraf et al, 2009; Zimmermann et al, 2012). Using functional MRI, Zentgraf et al (2009) examined neural activation differences between groups performing practiced finger sequence movements under external or internal focus instructions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Behavioral effects have dominated the attentional focus literature. Some, although more limited, attention has been paid to potential neural or brain-related underpinnings of the EF advantage, particularly those in motor cortex and somatosensory areas (Kuhn et al, 2018; Kuhn et al, 2017; Suzuki & Meehan, 2020; Zentgraf et al, 2009; Zimmermann et al, 2012). Using functional MRI, Zentgraf et al (2009) examined neural activation differences between groups performing practiced finger sequence movements under external or internal focus instructions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Disruptions to efficient goal-action coupling in IF conditions might be explained by more conscious constraint of the motor system in attempting to control movement processes (Wulf et al, 2001), with concomitant additional demand for coordination of these processes to produce a coherent response. Alternatively, or complementarily, an EF may be preventative, reducing accessibility to afferent information (Suzuki & Meehan, 2020), potential self-referential processing, or activation of a self-invoking trigger associated with (IF) references to one’s own body (McKay et al, 2015; Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2010; Yin et al, 2019). Increased confidence that accrues with effective performance and ease of movement may result in additional performance and learning benefits (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, modulation of sensorimotor inhibition may also stem from the engagement of attentional circuits. Indeed, paying attention to the body part tested in the SAI protocol (hand) during an attentional task (internal focus) leads to a greater SAI with respect to what observed when the focus is external 69 . Such an effect was explained by observing that internal focus might result in an augmentation of somatosensory volleys from the specific body part, which is in keeping with the notion that observing and paying attention to a specific body part enhance the somatosensory representation of that body part 70 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…46 An external focus of attention may be successful by facilitating functional connectivity, modulating surround inhibition, and increasing intracortical inhibition to resemble the sensorimotor integration typically seen in more skilled action. [47][48][49] The constrained action hypothesis suggests that an internal focus disrupts the motor system by promoting conscious control which leads to a breakdown in the in the otherwise natural organized coordination of one's movement. On the other hand, an external focus allows for a greater degree of self-organization by promoting automatic control processes.…”
Section: Considering An External Focus Of Attention To Optimize Movement Outcomementioning
confidence: 99%