2015
DOI: 10.1007/7854_2015_5009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attention and the Cholinergic System: Relevance to Schizophrenia

Abstract: Traditional methods of drug discovery often rely on a unidirectional, "bottom-up" approach: A search for molecular compounds that target a particular neurobiological substrate (e.g., a receptor type), the refinement of those compounds, testing in animal models using high-throughput behavioral screening methods, and then human testing for safety and effectiveness. Many attempts have found the "effectiveness" criterion to be a major stumbling block, and we and others have suggested that success may be improved b… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
30
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 185 publications
1
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Instead, cortical, and especially fronto-parietal, cholinergic activity are hypothesized to support “attentional effort” – the motivated activation of top-down attention to stabilize task representations, especially when performance is challenged (Lustig & Sarter, 2016; Raizada & Poldrack, 2008; Sarter et al, 2006). Fronto-parietal cholinergic activity increases reliably from baseline to the SAT, and further in response to the dSAT condition or other challenges (see Kozak et al, 2006); even placing trained animals in the operant chamber or simply testing them at the same time every day can lead to increases in frontoparietal cholinergic activity suggesting a readiness to engage in the task (Paolone et al, 2012).…”
Section: 1 Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, cortical, and especially fronto-parietal, cholinergic activity are hypothesized to support “attentional effort” – the motivated activation of top-down attention to stabilize task representations, especially when performance is challenged (Lustig & Sarter, 2016; Raizada & Poldrack, 2008; Sarter et al, 2006). Fronto-parietal cholinergic activity increases reliably from baseline to the SAT, and further in response to the dSAT condition or other challenges (see Kozak et al, 2006); even placing trained animals in the operant chamber or simply testing them at the same time every day can lead to increases in frontoparietal cholinergic activity suggesting a readiness to engage in the task (Paolone et al, 2012).…”
Section: 1 Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Degeneration of the cholinergic system therefore is expected to disrupt both the learning of associations between signals and the response rules needed to interact with the outside world and the use of (learned) signals to guide the retrieval of associative frameworks guiding action selection (e.g., Mesulam, 2004; Sarter, Albin, Kucinski, & Lustig, 2014). Furthermore, ill-timed cholinergic transients, due to abnormalities in the organization of corticosubcortical circuitry, may cause false detections or inappropriate attention and response to behaviorally-insignificant signals and therefore support psychopathological states (Lustig & Sarter, 2016). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Effort has been defined as the motivated activation of task-set representations in response to reduced performance or other indicators of increased task difficulty due to external or internal factors (e.g., the flickering described above, fatigue, advanced age) (Sarter, Gehring, & Kozak, 2006). The right prefrontal cortex (RPFC) has been proposed as a critical locus for interactions between control, motivation, and effort (e.g., Lustig & Sarter, 2016; Watanabe & Sakagami, 2007). However, as Botvinick and Braver note, the neural correlates of effort remain relatively unexplored.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Supporting the role of BA 9 in directing these functions, it is highly connected with sensory, motor, parietal, and other PFC regions as well as midbrain and limbic structures (Alexander, DeLong, & Strick, 1986; Bates & Goldman-Rakic, 1993; Miller & Cohen, 2001). In a recent review, we noted that it serves as a common locus for increased activation in response to control demands in young adults, putatively compensational increases in activation during control-demanding tasks in older adults, and abnormal function during working memory tasks in schizophrenia (Lustig & Sarter, 2016, their Figure 3). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation