2005
DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.11.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attention and prepulse inhibition: the effects of task-relevant, irrelevant, and no-task conditions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

4
39
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
4
39
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It has been known that the amplitude of the acoustic startle reflex is enhanced when a stimulus previously paired with footshock is presented before the startling stimulus (e.g., Brown 2006; Bradley et al, 1993;Filion & Poje, 2003;Grillon & Davis, 1997;Heekeren et al, 2004;Schell et al, 2000;Thorne et al, 2005). Previous studies have shown that rats are able to detect a correlation change between sounds delivered from two spatially separated locations (J.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It has been known that the amplitude of the acoustic startle reflex is enhanced when a stimulus previously paired with footshock is presented before the startling stimulus (e.g., Brown 2006; Bradley et al, 1993;Filion & Poje, 2003;Grillon & Davis, 1997;Heekeren et al, 2004;Schell et al, 2000;Thorne et al, 2005). Previous studies have shown that rats are able to detect a correlation change between sounds delivered from two spatially separated locations (J.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In humans, selective attention to the prepulse enhances PPI (e.g., Filion & Poje, 2003;Heekeren, Meincke, Geyer, & Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, 2004;Schell, Wynn, Dawson, Sinaii, & Niebala, 2000;Thorne, Dawsona, & Schell, 2005), and PPI is more pronounced when the prepulse is emotionally salient than when it is a neutral stimulus (Bradley, Codispoti, & Lang, 2006;Bradley, Cuthbert, & Lang, 1993). In rats with normal rearing, when the prepulse becomes fear conditioned (J.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ropinirole-induced PPI disruption at the 80-but not 50-ms lead interval is perhaps best explained by a DRD 3 -mediated reduction of PFC DA levels; one corollary of this proposal is that 50 ms would be too short an interval for cognitive "top-down" influences from the PFC to fully exert their modulatory effect on the subcortical startle circuit. Indeed, independent evidence from attention-to-prepulse paradigms has shown that intervals up to 60 ms, but not lower, may be prone to selective attention effects (Thorne et al 2005), and we have shown that performance in PFC-dependent tasks correlates with PPI at 80 ms lead interval Bitsios et al 2006). Moreover, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown that PPI at longer (120 ms), but not shorter (30 ms), lead intervals is associated with cortical activation (Kumari et al 2006).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Considering that dopamine plays an important role in the regulation of attentional processing (e.g., Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999;Nieoullon, 2002), and that increasing attention to the prepulse increase PPI (e.g., Elden, & Flaten, 2002;Dawson, Hazlett, Filion, Nuechterlein, & Schell, 1993;Thorne, Dawson, & Schell, 2005), the observed reduction of PPI during the induction of a positive Finally, the reported data are also relevant because they can contribute to the understanding of startle and PPI modulation as a function of clinical pathologies. Thus, it has been reported deficits in PPI in schizophrenia patients (e.g., Braff, Swerdlow, & Geyer, 1999;Braff, Geyer, Light, Sprock, Perry, Cadenhead, & Swerdlow, 2001), in obsessive-compulsive patients (Hoenig, Hochrein, Quednow, and Wagner, 2005), in panic disorder patients (Ludewig, Geyer, Ramseier, Vollenweider, Rechsteiner, & Cattapan-Ludewig, 2005), or in bipolar disorder patients (Perry, Minassian, Feifel, and Braff, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%