2020
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/ruwp9
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Asymmetries in visual acuity around the visual field

Abstract: Human vision is heterogeneous across the visual field. At a fixed eccentricity, performance is better along the horizontal than the vertical meridian, and better along the lower than the upper vertical meridian. These asymmetric patterns, termed performance fields, have been found in numerous visual tasks, including those mediated by contrast sensitivity and spatial resolution. However, it is unknown whether spatial resolution asymmetries are confined to the cardinal meridia or whether, and how far, they exten… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
69
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3
3

Relationship

4
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(82 citation statements)
references
References 85 publications
(153 reference statements)
12
69
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, performance fields asymmetries for SF sensitivity also weaken with increasing angular distance from the VM (Barbot et al, 2020). Notably, these studies are yet to measure contrastdefined performance fields across different stimulus eccentricities or sizes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly, performance fields asymmetries for SF sensitivity also weaken with increasing angular distance from the VM (Barbot et al, 2020). Notably, these studies are yet to measure contrastdefined performance fields across different stimulus eccentricities or sizes.…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Likewise, performance is better along the lower vertical meridian (LVM) than the upper vertical meridian (UVM) -a phenomenon termed the Vertical-Meridian Asymmetry (VMA) (Carrasco, Talgar, & Cameron, 2001). Together these asymmetries are called performance fields and have been identified for a range of tasks involving contrast sensitivity (Abrams, Nizam, & Carrasco, 2012;Baldwin et al, 2012;Cameron, Tai, & Carrasco, 2002;Levine & McAnany, 2005;Lundh, Lennerstrand, & Derefeldt, 1983;Pointer & Hess, 1989;Regan & Beverley, 1983;Rijsdijk, Kroon, & van der Wildt, 1980;Robson & Graham, 1981;Silva et al, 2008), contrast appearance (Fuller, Rodriguez, & Carrasco, 2008), spatial resolution (Barbot, Xue, & Carrasco, 2020;Carrasco, Williams, & Yeshurun, 2002;Talgar & Carrasco, 2002), temporal information accrual (Carrasco, Giordano, & McElree, 2004), crowding (Fortenbaugh, Silver, & Robertson, 2015;Greenwood, Szinte, Sayim, & Cavanagh, 2017), visual short term memory (Montaser-Kouhsari & Carrasco, 2009), and motion perception (Fuller & Carrasco, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[73]). This nasal-temporal asymmetry, although interesting, is beyond the focus of this paper, as the asymmetries in performance we observe are found in both binocular and monocular experiments [10, 32]. Overall, the greater emphasis on the horizontal is substantially greater in the mRGCs than the cones.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 57%
“…The asymmetries gradually fall-off with angular distance from the meridians [74]. This gradual decrease in radial asymmetry in cortex parallels the gradual decrease in contrast sensitivity [10, 25, 26] and spatial frequency sensitivity [32] with angular distance from the cardinal meridians. Measurements of cone density and retinal ganglion cell density have emphasized the meridians, so there is less information regarding how the asymmetries vary with angular distance from the meridians.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation