2019
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00883
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Assessing the Overlap Between Three Measures of Food Reward

Abstract: Food reward is an important concept for research in eating behaviors. Many food reward tasks have been developed and are in active use. However, little is known how much these tasks overlap. Here, we sought to compare three promising food reward tasks: (1) the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire (LFPQ; a procedure combining explicit ratings of wanting and liking and an implicit wanting task based on forced choice), (2) a hand grip force task, and (3) an emotional attentional blink (EAB) task. Specifically, we … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
13
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
(64 reference statements)
2
13
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The handgrip task, which we believed would track changes in reward value of the beverage over the exposure period, also showed no significant group differences based on calories or viscosity. Interestingly, in agreement with (Arumäe et al, (2019), we found that the behavioural measure of wanting (i.e., the hand grip task) was positively associated with self-report measures of wanting (rs = .3-.5). We also observed group differences in liking for the beveragethose with high viscosity were less liked than those with low viscosity, though liking was not altered by exposure to calories.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The handgrip task, which we believed would track changes in reward value of the beverage over the exposure period, also showed no significant group differences based on calories or viscosity. Interestingly, in agreement with (Arumäe et al, (2019), we found that the behavioural measure of wanting (i.e., the hand grip task) was positively associated with self-report measures of wanting (rs = .3-.5). We also observed group differences in liking for the beveragethose with high viscosity were less liked than those with low viscosity, though liking was not altered by exposure to calories.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…In this way, handgrip force is argued to be a measure of beverage reward value (Ziauddeen et al, 2014). The hand grip force task taps into the same construct as liking and wanting (Arumäe et al, 2019), but has not been applied previously in the context of dietary learning.…”
Section: Grip Force Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Adaptations of the LFPQ have included dimensions of protein (Griffioen-Roose et al, 2011;Karl et al, 2018), fruits/vegetables and snacks (G. Finlayson, N. King, & J. Blundell, 2007b), and alcoholic/soft drinks in high or low calorie form (unpublished data). The LFPQ has also been used in different appetite-related contexts such as high altitude (Aeberli et al, 2013), elderly care homes (Van der Meij, Wijnhoven, Finlayson, Oosten, & Visser, 2015), eating disorder clinics (Cowdrey, Finlayson, & Park, 2013;Dalton & Finlayson, 2014), sleep laboratories (McNeil et al, 2017), bariatric surgery wards (Redpath et al, 2018), or anti-obesity/diabetes drug trials (Blundell et al, 2017) and is now translated linguistically into 16 languages including Tamil (Ranasinghe et al, 2018), Arabic (Alkahtni, Dalton, Abuzaid, Obeid, & Finlayson, 2016), Mandarin Chinese (Zhou et al, 2019), Estonian (Arumäe, Kreegipuu, & Vainik, 2019) and Norwegian (Martins et al, 2017). The widespread use and adaptation of the LFPQ creates a need to provide a uniform procedure and best practice recommendations to develop and implement reliable cultural adaptations, improve data quality and facilitate comparison with other studies.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The most common Food Reward measures are self-reported liking [16,17], self-reported desire-to-eat [13] or wanting for a specific food [13,16]. Many Food Reward measures and tasks have been developed and are in active use [18]. Rogers and Hardman (2015) define Food Reward as the "momentary value of a food to the individual at the time of ingestion", measured directly with a rating of "desire to eat the entire portion right now" [19].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The LFPQ has been applied and/or adapted by several researchers [16,20,[22][23][24]. Additional Food Reward measures include grip force operant tasks [18,20,25], willingness to pay [20,26], and Emotional attentional blink [18,27]. An operant task could include tapping a space bar for 60 seconds, being told, the more you tap, the more of a given food you will receive [19,20], or by squeezing a handheld dynamometer as a response to specific food images [25].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%