2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2018.09.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Assessing the background decomposition of a complex auditory scene with event-related brain potentials

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
2
1
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…And if so, are these predictions utilized to promote behavioral goals? Indeed, the auditory system does seem to be sensitive to the timing of unattended sounds, as reflected, for example, by Mismatch Negativity (MMN) responses to interval and duration deviants (Lyytinen et al 1992;Escera et al 2002;Winkler et al 2003;Kisley et al 2004;Zion-Golumbic et al 2007;Lumaca et al 2018;Thomassen and Bendixen 2018). (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…And if so, are these predictions utilized to promote behavioral goals? Indeed, the auditory system does seem to be sensitive to the timing of unattended sounds, as reflected, for example, by Mismatch Negativity (MMN) responses to interval and duration deviants (Lyytinen et al 1992;Escera et al 2002;Winkler et al 2003;Kisley et al 2004;Zion-Golumbic et al 2007;Lumaca et al 2018;Thomassen and Bendixen 2018). (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The copyright holder for this preprint this version posted January 28, 2020. ; https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.01.27.920728 doi: bioRxiv preprint Besides being important for auditory stream segregation (Nelken 2004;Näätänen et al 2010;Thomassen and Bendixen 2018), forming representations for the temporal structure of distractor sounds also seem to bear a functional role in facilitating selective attention (Sussman 2017). For example, several behavioral studies have shown that performance on selective-attention auditory tasks was less disrupted by the presence of rhythmic vs. random auditory distractors (Devergie et al 2010), at least in young adults (Rimmele et al 2012) and when the task was sufficiently difficult (Andreou et al 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…And if so, are these predictions utilized to promote behavioral goals? Indeed, the auditory system does seem to be sensitive to the timing of unattended sounds, as reflected, for example, by Mismatch Negativity (MMN) responses to interval and duration deviants (Lyytinen et al 1992;Escera et al 2002;Winkler et al 2003;Kisley et al 2004;Zion-Golumbic et al 2007;Lumaca et al 2018; Thomassen and Bendixen 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Besides being important for auditory stream segregation (Nelken 2004;Näätänen et al 2010; Thomassen and Bendixen 2018), forming representations for the temporal structure of distractor sounds also seem to bear a functional role in facilitating selective attention (Sussman 2017). For example, several behavioral studies have shown that performance on selective-attention auditory tasks was less disrupted by the presence of rhythmic vs. random auditory distractors (Devergie et al 2010), at least in young adults (Rimmele et al 2012) and when the task was sufficiently difficult (Andreou et al 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%