2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.11.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Assessing preference and reinforcer effectiveness in dogs

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

5
51
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
5
51
0
Order By: Relevance
“…touching or pushing various stimuli) in giant pandas ( Ailuropoda melanoleuca ) and African elephants (Loxodonta Africana ). Similarly, Vicars et al (2014) found that items identified as highly preferred by a paired-choice task in domestic dogs ( Canis lupus familiaris ) subsequently served as effective reinforcers in a progressive ratio procedure in which the task was touching the experimenter’s fist with the dog’s nose. However, it is unknown whether items identified by preference techniques will translate to faster learning or more efficient training sessions when training animals for more complex tasks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…touching or pushing various stimuli) in giant pandas ( Ailuropoda melanoleuca ) and African elephants (Loxodonta Africana ). Similarly, Vicars et al (2014) found that items identified as highly preferred by a paired-choice task in domestic dogs ( Canis lupus familiaris ) subsequently served as effective reinforcers in a progressive ratio procedure in which the task was touching the experimenter’s fist with the dog’s nose. However, it is unknown whether items identified by preference techniques will translate to faster learning or more efficient training sessions when training animals for more complex tasks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Fisher et al (1992) found that this forcedchoice procedure resulted in greater differentiation among stimuli and also was a better predictor of which stimuli would subsequently serve as effective reinforcers. Paired-choice procedures are widely used to determine food, toy, or activity preferences in humans with developmental disorders (e.g., DeLeon et al, 2001; Fisher et al, 1992; Piazza et al, 1996) and have also been used to determine food preferences for a variety of animal species, ranging from great apes to possums (e.g., Benz et al, 1992; Cameron et al, 2013; Clay et al, 2009; Fernandez et al, 2004; Gaalema et al, 2011; Harlow & Myer, 1952; Mehrkam & Dorey, 2014; 2015; Polidora & Schneider, 1964; Remis, 2000; Vicars et al, 2014). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition, rewarding desired behaviour is reported to strengthen the human-animal bond (Deldalle and Gaunet 2014;Payne, Bennett and McGreevy 2015), decrease animal stress during training (Deldalle and Gaunet 2014) and mitigate the development of problematic behaviour which can result in relinquishment of the dog by the owner if it persists (Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey 2008). To successfully implement positive reinforcement, one must use a functional reinforcer; this means that the reinforcer is highly valued by the animal (Gaalema, Perdue and Kelling 2011;Vicars, Miguel and Sobie 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A well-researched method used for assessing preference with animals is the paired stimulus preference assessment (e.g., Cameron et al 2013;Clay et al 2009) as it requires little effort on the part of the researcher, does not involve large amounts of food and is relatively quick to administer (Vicars et al 2014;Riemer et al 2018;). It is also a valid and reliable method for assessing preference and involves pairs of food or items being systematically offered to the subject with their choice recorded (Fisher et al 1992) and is reliable over time (e.g., Cameron et al 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%