2014
DOI: 10.1111/joes.12069
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Are Women Really More Risk‐averse Than Men? A Re‐analysis of the Literature Using Expanded Methods

Abstract: Abstract. While a substantial literature in economics and finance has concluded that 'women are more risk averse than men', this conclusion merits investigation. After briefly clarifying the difference between making generalizations about groups, on the one hand, and making valid inferences from samples, on the other, this essay suggests improvements to how economists communicate our research results. Supplementing findings of statistical significance with quantitative measures of both substantive difference (… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
112
1
3

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 195 publications
(123 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
7
112
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…4 However, the evidence for gender differences in risk preferences is not that clear (Niederle, 2016). See Nelson (2014) for a critique on generalizing statements. 5 Niederle & Vesterlund (2011) discuss three elicitation methods: (1) revealing risk preferences from choices in settings that do not involve any competition but involve a similar risk; (2) eliciting risk preferences from incentivized lotteries; or (3) from drawing upon stated risk preferences from a questionnaire.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…4 However, the evidence for gender differences in risk preferences is not that clear (Niederle, 2016). See Nelson (2014) for a critique on generalizing statements. 5 Niederle & Vesterlund (2011) discuss three elicitation methods: (1) revealing risk preferences from choices in settings that do not involve any competition but involve a similar risk; (2) eliciting risk preferences from incentivized lotteries; or (3) from drawing upon stated risk preferences from a questionnaire.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3 Thus, in order to determine the participants' self-confidence in a setting that is free from potential gender bias, we implement a 2 See Niederle & Vesterlund (2007), Niederle & Vesterlund (2011) and Kamas & Preston (2012). For a critique on generalizing statements concerning gender differences see Nelson (2014).…”
Section: Literature Review and Conjecturesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A review and analysis by Nelson (2015), however, finds the results to be more mixed, with some studies reporting higher female average risk taking and many cases in which the male advantage lacked statistical significance. more risk averse among persons drawn from the general population or among university students, studies that focus on managers and professionals have found little or no evidence of gender differences in financial risk preferences.…”
Section: Risk Aversionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, if the analysis of two different sustainability initiatives for the reduction of CO 2 emissions finds results that are both "statistically significant", but the first initiative reduces emissions by 1%, while the second reduces them by 10%, then the latter is more effective for environmental protection having greater "substantive significance". For this reason, confusing these concepts can lead to systematic and dangerous biases in scientific research (Ziliak & McCloskey, 2008) and, consequently, to biased management and policy decisions, as has been shown in psychology (Hyde, 2005;Fine, 2010), economics (Filippin & Crosetto, 2014;Nelson, 2014) and many other different fields, from political sciences to medicine and pharmacology (Ziliak & McCloskey, 2008).…”
Section: The Difference Between Statistical Significance and Substantmentioning
confidence: 99%