2020
DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17124570
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Are the Effects of Variation in Quantity of Daily Bilingual Exposure and Socioeconomic Status on Language and Cognitive Abilities Independent in Preschool Children?

Abstract: Bilingual exposure (BE) and socioeconomic status (SES) are associated with children’s development, but their specific and unique effects are still unclear. This study analyzed the influence of these environmental factors on a set of cognitive and linguistic abilities in preschoolers to disentangle their effects. One hundred-eleven Italian-speaking preschool children (mean age = 61 months; SD = 6.8) growing in a monolingual or multilingual context completed an assessment of cognitive (theory of mind, inhibition… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
24
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 91 publications
0
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…EF was measured in 25 experiments within 21 articles, making it the most frequently tested cognitive ability in this age group ( Aktan-Erciyes, 2020 , Studies 1 and 2; Bain & Yu, 1980, Studies 1 and 2 ; Barac, Moreno, & Bialystok, 2016 ; Bialystok, 1999 ; Bialystok & Martin, 2004, Studies 1, 2, and 3 ; Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008 ; Crespo & Kaushanskaya, 2021 ; Diaz & Farrar, 2018a ; Diaz & Farrar, 2018b ; Dicataldo & Roch, 2020 ; Gathercole et al, 2016 ; Goldman, Negen, & Sarnecka, 2014 ; Haft et al, 2019 ; Leikin & Tovli, 2014 ; Mehrani & Zabihi, 2017 ; Namazi & Thordardottir, 2010 ; Nguyen & Astington, 2014 ; Tran, Arredondo, & Yoshida, 2015 ; Tran, Arredondo, & Yoshida, 2019 ; Yoshida, Tran, Benitez, & Kuwabara, 2010 ; Yoshida, Tran, Benitez, & Kuwabara, 2011 ).…”
Section: Preschool-age Studies (2;6 To 6 Years Of Age)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…EF was measured in 25 experiments within 21 articles, making it the most frequently tested cognitive ability in this age group ( Aktan-Erciyes, 2020 , Studies 1 and 2; Bain & Yu, 1980, Studies 1 and 2 ; Barac, Moreno, & Bialystok, 2016 ; Bialystok, 1999 ; Bialystok & Martin, 2004, Studies 1, 2, and 3 ; Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008 ; Crespo & Kaushanskaya, 2021 ; Diaz & Farrar, 2018a ; Diaz & Farrar, 2018b ; Dicataldo & Roch, 2020 ; Gathercole et al, 2016 ; Goldman, Negen, & Sarnecka, 2014 ; Haft et al, 2019 ; Leikin & Tovli, 2014 ; Mehrani & Zabihi, 2017 ; Namazi & Thordardottir, 2010 ; Nguyen & Astington, 2014 ; Tran, Arredondo, & Yoshida, 2015 ; Tran, Arredondo, & Yoshida, 2019 ; Yoshida, Tran, Benitez, & Kuwabara, 2010 ; Yoshida, Tran, Benitez, & Kuwabara, 2011 ).…”
Section: Preschool-age Studies (2;6 To 6 Years Of Age)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One of these articles tested both preschool-aged and adolescent participants (Gathercole, Thomas, Viñas Guasch, Kennedy, Prys, Young, et al, 2016), and is also discussed in the adolescent section of our Results. As summarized in Barac, Moreno, & Bialystok, 2016;Bialystok, 1999;Bialystok & Martin, 2004, Studies 1, 2, and 3; Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008;Diaz & Farrar, 2018a;Diaz & Farrar, 2018b;Dicataldo & Roch, 2020;Gathercole et al, 2016;Goldman, Negen, & Sarnecka, 2014;Haft et al, 2019;Leikin & Tovli, 2014;Mehrani & Zabihi, 2017;Namazi & Thordardottir, 2010;Nguyen & Astington, 2014;Tran, Arredondo, & Yoshida, 2015;Tran, Arredondo, & Yoshida, 2019;Yoshida, Tran, Benitez, & Kuwabara, 2010;Yoshida, Tran, Benitez, & Kuwabara, 2011).…”
Section: Target Cognitive Abilities and Cognitive Tasks Usedmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Inhibitory control and response inhibition were assessed in 14 experiments using the child ANT (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008), a visually cued recall task (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008), the Kansas Reflection/Impulsivity Scale (KRISP; Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008), the Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (C-TONI; Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008), the Statue task (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008), the Delay of Gratification task (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008), Stroop and 'Strooplike' (Day/Night, Happy/Sad) tasks (Diaz & Farrar, 2018a;Diaz & Farrar, 2018b;Dicataldo & Roch, 2020;Nguyen & Astington, 2014;Tran et al, 2019), Simon Says and Bear/Dragon Simon Says tasks (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008;Diaz & Farrar, 2018a;Diaz & Farrar, 2018b;Tran et al, 2019), the Simon task ('interference suppression', Gathercole et al, 2016;Mehrani & Zabihi, 2017), the Gift delay task (Barac et al, 2016;Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008;Tran et al, 2019), a Go/No-Go task (Barac et al, 2016), and a non-symbolic numerical discrimination task (Goldman et al, 2014). The Dimensional Change Card Sort task (DCCS) was also used to measure inhibition (Aktan-Erciyes, 2020, Studies 1 and 2; Bialystok, 1999;Bialystok & Martin, 2004, Studies 1, 2, and 3;Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008;Tran et al, 2019); this task was additionally used as a measure of cognitive flexibility in three experiments (Diaz & Farrar, 2018a;Diaz & Farrar, 2018b;Haft et al, 2019), shifting in five experiments ('switching', Aktan-Erciyes, 2020, Studies BILINGUAL CHILDHOOD COGNITIVE MEASURES 20 1 and 2; 'attention-shifting', Dicataldo & Roch, 2020...…”
Section: Bilingual Childhood Cognitive Measures 19mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations