2013
DOI: 10.1037/a0031672
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Are stimulus–response rules represented phonologically for task-set preparation and maintenance?

Abstract: Accounts of task-set control generally assume that the current task's stimulus-response (S-R) rules must be elevated to a privileged state of activation. How are they represented in this state? In 3 task-cuing experiments, we tested the hypothesis that phonological working memory is used to represent S-R rules for task-set control by getting participants to switch between 2 sets of arbitrary S-R rules and manipulating the articulatory duration (Experiment 1) or phonological similarity (Experiments 2 and 3) of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
18
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
2
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In relation to previous findings, our results are consistent with those of Van 't Wout et al (2013), who found that S-R rules are not represented phonologically once the task is well-practiced.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In relation to previous findings, our results are consistent with those of Van 't Wout et al (2013), who found that S-R rules are not represented phonologically once the task is well-practiced.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…Miyake, Emerson, Padilla, & Ahn, 2004). Finally, one other task switching study designed to investigate the contribution of language to task-set control manipulated the phonological similarity of the stimulus terms (Van 't Wout, Lavric, & Monsell, 2013). This study did not find any effect of phonological similarity on task switching performance once the tasks were well-practiced.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 87%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These studies hence support the conclusion that maintaining a verbal goal representation plays an important role in task switching: when the cues are transparent, the verbal goal can be retrieved without any additional verbal processing, but when the cues are absent or nontransparent, additional verbal processing is needed to establish the verbal goal. 2 Inner speech may be used to support this maintenance, and it takes the form of verbalization of task goals, task cues, and possibly category-response mappings (Liefooghe et al, 2005; but see van’t Wout, Lavric, & Monsell, 2013). Thus inner speech supports cue interpretation, particularly in situations in which the cues are arbitrary and do not provide direct access to the task name or the task goal (Miyake et al, 2004; Saeki & Saito, 2012).…”
Section: Empirical Constraints On the Wm Architecturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, switching and working memory appear to be related to two separate individual-differences dimensions (Miyake & Friedman, 2012). Furthermore, experimental work employing working-memory load manipulations show minimal if any involvement of working memory in task switching (Kessler & Meiran, 2009; Kiesel, Wendt, & Peters, 2005; van’t Wout, Lavric, & Monsell, 2013). Additionally, in the Meiran et al (2002) study, participants switched between highly practiced tasks, such that the potential absence of proactive processes may be selective to non-RITL contexts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%